Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 22, Issue 2, pp 437–444 | Cite as

Interactions between voluntary and involuntary attention modulate the quality and temporal dynamics of visual processing

  • Michael A. Grubb
  • Alex L. White
  • David J. Heeger
  • Marisa Carrasco
Brief Report

Abstract

Successfully navigating a dynamic environment requires the efficient distribution of finite neural resources. Voluntary (endogenous) covert spatial attention selectively allocates those processing resources to goal-relevant locations in the visual scene in the absence of eye movements. However, the allocation of spatial attention is not always voluntary; abrupt onsets in the visual periphery automatically enhance processing of nearby stimuli (exogenous attention). In dynamic environments, exogenous events and internal goals likely compete to determine the distribution of attention, but how such competition is resolved is not well understood. To investigate how exogenous events interact with the concurrent allocation of voluntary attention, we used a speed–accuracy trade-off (SAT) procedure. SAT conjointly measures the rate of information accrual and asymptotic discriminability, allowing us to measure how attentional interactions unfold over time during stimulus processing. We found that both types of attention sped information accrual and improved discriminability. However, focusing endogenous attention at the target location reduced the effects of exogenous cues on the rate of information accrual and rendered negligible their effects on asymptotic discriminability. We verified the robustness of these findings in four additional experiments that targeted specific, critical response delays. In conclusion, the speed and quality of visual processing depend conjointly on internally and externally driven attentional states, but it is possible to voluntarily diminish distraction by irrelevant events in the periphery.

Keywords

Covert spatial attention Speed-acccuracy trade-off Visual perception Psychophysics 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH grant R01-EY019693 to D.H. and M.C. and by T32 EY007136 to New York University. We thank members of the Carrasco Lab for helpful comments.

Supplementary material

13423_2014_698_MOESM1_ESM.doc (44 kb)
Supplementary Online Materials (DOC 43.5 kb)

References

  1. Anton-Erxleben, K., Abrams, J., & Carrasco, M. (2010). Evaluating comparative and equality judgments in contrast perception: attention alters appearance. Journal of Vision, 10(11): 6, 1–22Google Scholar
  2. Anton-Erxleben, K., Abrams, J., & Carrasco, M. (2011). Equality judgments cannot distinguish between attention effects on appearance and criterion: a reply to Schneider (2011). Journal of Vision, 11(13):8, 1–8.Google Scholar
  3. Barbot, A., Landy, M. S., & Carrasco, M. (2011). Exogenous attention enhances 2nd-order contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 51(9), 1086–1098.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Berger, A., Henik, A., & Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 207–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Busse, L., Katzner, S., & Treue, S. (2008). Temporal dynamics of neuronal modulation during exogenous and endogenous shifts of visual attention in macaque area MT. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 105(42), 16380–16385.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Carrasco, M. (2009). Attention: Psychophysical approaches. In T. Bayne, A. Cleeremans, & P. Wilken (Eds.), The Oxford companion to consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: the past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13), 1484–1525.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Carrasco, M., Giordano, A. M., & McElree, B. (2004). Temporal performance fields: visual and attentional factors. Vision Research, 44(12), 1351–1365.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Carrasco, M., Giordano, A. M., & McElree, B. (2006). Attention speeds processing across eccentricity: feature and conjunction searches. Vision Research, 46(13), 2028–2040.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Carrasco, M., & McElree, B. (2001). Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 98(9), 5363–5367.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Carrasco, M., McElree, B., Denisova, K., & Giordano, A. M. (2003). Speed of visual processing increases with eccentricity. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 669–670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chica, A. B., Bartolomeo, P., & Lupianez, J. (2013). Two cognitive and neural systems for endogenous and exogenous spatial attention. Behavioral Brain Research, 237, 107–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dosher, B. A., Han, S., & Lu, Z. L. (2004). Parallel processing in visual search asymmetry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(1), 3–27.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Giordano, A. M., McElree, B., & Carrasco, M. (2009). On the automaticity and flexibility of covert attention: a speed-accuracy trade-off analysis. Journal of Vision, 9(3), 30 1–10.Google Scholar
  16. Herrmann, K., Montaser-Kouhsari, L., Carrasco, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). When size matters: attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nature Neuroscience, 13(12), 1554–1559.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hopfinger, J. B., & West, V. M. (2006). Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing. Neuroimage, 31(2), 774–789.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Kimchi, R., & Peterson, M. A. (2008). Figure-ground segmentation can occur without attention. Psychological Science, 19(7), 660–668.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). When sustained attention impairs perception. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1243–1245.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. McElree, B. (1996). Accessing short-term memory with semantic and phonological information: A time-course analysis. Memory & Cognition, 24, 173–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McElree, B., & Dosher, B. A. (1989). Serial position and set size in short-term memory: Time course of recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 18, 346–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. McElree, B., Murphy, G. L., & Ochoa, T. (2006). Time course of retrieving conceptual information: a speed-accuracy trade-off study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(5), 848–853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Montagna, B., Pestilli, F., & Carrasco, M. (2009). Attention trades off spatial acuity. Vision Research, 49(7), 735–745.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Muller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(2), 315–330.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Nakayama, K., & Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vision Research, 29(11), 1631–1647.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Pestilli, F., Carrasco, M., Heeger, D. J., & Gardner, J. L. (2011). Attentional enhancement via selection and pooling of early sensory responses in human visual cortex. Neuron, 72(5), 832–846.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Reed, A. V. (1973). Speed-accuracy trade-off in recognition memory. Science, 181(4099), 574–576.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Reynolds, J. H., & Heeger, D. J. (2009). The normalization model of attention. Neuron, 61(2), 168–185.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Schneider, K. A. (2011). Attention alters decision criteria but not appearance: a reanalysis of Anton-Erxleben, Abrams, and Carrasco (2010). Journal of Vision, 11(13): 7, 1–8.Google Scholar
  30. Schneider, K. A., & Komlos, M. (2008). Attention biases decisions but does not alter appearance. Journal of Vision, 8(15):3, 1–10.Google Scholar
  31. Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: the effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception and Psychophysics, 49(1), 83–90.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. van der Lubbe, R. H., & Postma, A. (2005). Interruption from irrelevant auditory and visual onsets even when attention is in a focused state. Experimental Brain Research, 164(4), 464–471.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Wickelgren, W. A. (1977). Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics. Acta Psychologica, 41(1), 67–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(1), 121–134.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Yeshurun, Y., Montagna, B., & Carrasco, M. (2008). On the flexibility of sustained attention and its effects on a texture segmentation task. Vision Research, 48(1), 80–95.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michael A. Grubb
    • 1
    • 2
  • Alex L. White
    • 2
  • David J. Heeger
    • 1
    • 2
  • Marisa Carrasco
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Center for Neural ScienceNew York UniversityNew YorkUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyNew York UniversityNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations