Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 21, Issue 6, pp 1431–1443 | Cite as

Generalized outcome-based strategy classification: Comparing deterministic and probabilistic choice models

  • Benjamin E. Hilbig
  • Morten Moshagen
Article

Abstract

Model comparisons are a vital tool for disentangling which of several strategies a decision maker may have used—that is, which cognitive processes may have governed observable choice behavior. However, previous methodological approaches have been limited to models (i.e., decision strategies) with deterministic choice rules. As such, psychologically plausible choice models—such as evidence-accumulation and connectionist models—that entail probabilistic choice predictions could not be considered appropriately. To overcome this limitation, we propose a generalization of Bröder and Schiffer’s (Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 361–380, 2003) choice-based classification method, relying on (1) parametric order constraints in the multinomial processing tree framework to implement probabilistic models and (2) minimum description length for model comparison. The advantages of the generalized approach are demonstrated through recovery simulations and an experiment. In explaining previous methods and our generalization, we maintain a nontechnical focus—so as to provide a practical guide for comparing both deterministic and probabilistic choice models.

Keywords

Judgment and decision making Model comparison Strategy classification Multinomial processing tree models Minimum description length 

Supplementary material

13423_2014_643_MOESM1_ESM.zip (5 kb)
ESM 1 (ZIP 5 KB)

References

  1. Andrews, D. W. K. (2000). Inconsistency of the bootstrap when a parameter is on the boundary of the parameter space. Econometrica, 68, 399–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1999). Theoretical and empirical review of multinomial process tree modeling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 57–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beach, L. R., & Mitchell, T. R. (1978). A contingency model for the selection of decision strategies. Academy of Management Review, 3, 439–449.Google Scholar
  4. Birnbaum, M. H., & Jou, J.-W. (1990). A theory of comparative response times and “difference” judgments. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 184–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bröder, A. (2000). Assessing the empirical validity of the “Take-the-best” heuristic as a model of human probabilistic inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1332–1346.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bröder, A. (2003). Decision making with the “adaptive toolbox”: Influence of environmental structure, intelligence, and working memory load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 611–625.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bröder, A. (2010). Outcome-based strategy classification. In A. Glöckner & C. Witteman (Eds.), Foundations for tracing intuition: Challenges and methods (pp. 61–82). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bröder, A., Newell, B. R., & Platzer, C. (2010). Cue integration vs. exemplar-based reasoning in multi-attribute decisions from memory: A matter of cue representation. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 326–338.Google Scholar
  9. Bröder, A., & Schiffer, S. (2003). Bayesian strategy assessment in multi-attribute decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 16, 193–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bröder, A., & Schiffer, S. (2006). Stimulus format and working memory in fast and frugal strategy selection. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 361–380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Broomell, S. B., Budescu, D. V., & Por, H.-H. (2011). Pair-wise comparisons of multiple models. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 821–831.Google Scholar
  12. Brown, N. R., & Tan, S. (2011). Magnitude comparison revisited: An alternative approach to binary choice under uncertainty. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 392–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Busemeyer, J. R., & Townsend, J. T. (1993). Decision field theory: A dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. Psychological Review, 100, 432–459.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chechile, R. A. (1998). A new method for estimating model parameters for multinomial data. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 42, 432–471.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dashiell, J. F. (1937). Affective value-distances as a determinant of esthetic judgment-times. The American Journal of Psychology, 50, 57–67.Google Scholar
  16. Davis-Stober, C. P. (2009). Analysis of multinomial models under inequality constraints: Applications to measurement theory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 53, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Davis-Stober, C. P., & Brown, N. (2011). A shift in strategy or “error”? Strategy classification over multiple stochastic specifications. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 800–813.Google Scholar
  18. Erdfelder, E., Auer, T.-S., Hilbig, B. E., Aßfalg, A., Moshagen, M., & Nadarevic, L. (2009). Multinomial processing tree models: A review of the literature. Zeitschrift für Psychologie - Journal of Psychology, 217, 108–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650–669.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gigerenzer, G., Hoffrage, U., & Kleinbölting, H. (1991). Probabilistic mental models: A Brunswikian theory of confidence. Psychological Review, 98, 506–528.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gigerenzer, G., & Selten, R. (2001). Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  22. Glöckner, A. (2009). Investigating intuitive and deliberate processes statistically: The multiple-measure maximum likelihood strategy classification method. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 186–199.Google Scholar
  23. Glöckner, A., & Betsch, T. (2008a). Do people make decisions under risk based on ignorance? An empirical test of the priority heuristic against cumulative prospect theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 107, 75–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Glöckner, A., & Betsch, T. (2008b). Modeling option and strategy choices with connectionist networks: Towards an integrative model of automatic and deliberate decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 215–228.Google Scholar
  25. Glöckner, A., & Betsch, T. (2008c). Multiple-reason decision making based on automatic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 1055–1075.Google Scholar
  26. Glöckner, A., & Betsch, T. (2012). Decisions beyond boundaries: When more information is processed faster than less. Acta Psychologica, 139, 532–542.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Glöckner, A., Betsch, T., & Schindler, N. (2010). Coherence shifts in probabilistic inference tasks. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23, 439–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Glöckner, A., & Bröder, A. (2011). Processing of recognition information and additional cues: A model-based analysis of choice, confidence, and response time. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 23–42.Google Scholar
  29. Glöckner, A., Fiedler, S., Hochman, G., Ayal, S., & Hilbig, B. E. (2012). Processing differences between descriptions and experience: A comparative analysis using eye-tracking and physiological measures. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 173.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Glöckner, A., & Herbold, A.-K. (2011). An eye-tracking study on information processing in risky decisions: Evidence for compensatory strategies based on automatic processes. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24, 71–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Glöckner, A., & Hodges, S. D. (2011). Parallel constraint satisfaction in memory-based decisions. Experimental Psychology, 58, 180–195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Glöckner, A., & Witteman, C. (2010). Beyond dual-process models: A categorization of processes underlying intuitive judgment and decision making. Thinking & Reasoning, 16, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Grünwald, P. D. (2007). The minimum description length principle. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  34. Heck, D. W., Moshagen, M., & Erdfelder, E. (2014). Model selection by minimum description length: Lower bound sample sizes for the Fisher information approximation. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  35. Hilbig, B. E. (2010a). Precise models deserve precise measures: A methodological dissection. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 272–284.Google Scholar
  36. Hilbig, B. E. (2010b). Reconsidering “evidence” for fast-and-frugal heuristics. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 923–930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hilbig, B. E., Erdfelder, E., & Pohl, R. F. (2011). Fluent, fast, and frugal? A formal model evaluation of the interplay between memory, fluency, and comparative judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 37, 827–839.Google Scholar
  38. Hilbig, B. E., Erdfelder, E., & Pohl, R. F. (2012). A matter of time: Antecedents of one-reason decision making based on recognition. Acta Psychologica, 141, 9–16.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Hilbig, B. E., & Glöckner, A. (2011). Yes, they can! Appropriate weighting of small probabilities as a function of information acquisition. Acta Psychologica, 138, 390–396.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Hu, X., & Batchelder, W. H. (1994). The statistical analysis of multinomial processing tree models with the EM algorithm. Psychometrika, 59, 21–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Jekel, M., Fiedler, S., & Glöckner, A. (2011). Diagnostic task selection for strategy classification in judgment and decision making: Theory, validation, and implementation in R. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 782–799.Google Scholar
  42. Jekel, M., & Glöckner, A. (2014). Doing Justice to Benjamin Franklin: Overestimation of the Use of Heuristics Due to Problematic Implementations of Weighted Compensatory Strategies. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  43. Jekel, M., Nicklisch, A., & Glöckner, A. (2010). Implementation of the Multiple-Measure Maximum Likelihood strategy classification method in R: Addendum to Glöckner (2009) and practical guide for application. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, 54–63.Google Scholar
  44. Johnson, E. J., Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., & Willemsen, M. C. (2008). Process models deserve process data: Comment on Brandstatter, Gigerenzer, and Hertwig (2006). Psychological Review, 115, 263–272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Klugkist, I., & Hoijtink, H. (2007). The Bayes factor for inequality and about inequality constrained models. Computational Statistics and Data Analysis, 51, 6367–6379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Knapp, B. R., & Batchelder, W. H. (2004). Representing parametric order constraints in multi-trial applications of multinomial processing tree models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 48, 215–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lee, M. D., & Cummins, T. D. (2004). Evidence accumulation in decision making: Unifying the “take the best” and the “rational” models. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 343–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Luce, R. D. (1977). The choice axiom after twenty years. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 15, 215–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Moshagen, M. (2010). multiTree: A computer program for the analysis of multinomial processing tree models. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 42–54.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Moshagen, M., & Hilbig, B. E. (2011). Methodological notes on model comparisons and strategy classification: A falsificationist proposition. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 814–820.Google Scholar
  51. Mosteller, F., & Nogee, P. (1951). An experimental measurement of utility. Journal of Political Economy, 59, 371–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Moyer, R. S., & Bayer, R. H. (1976). Mental comparison and the symbolic distance effect. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 228–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Myung, I. J. (2000). The importance of complexity in model selection. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 44, 190–204.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Myung, I. J., Navarro, D. J., & Pitt, M. A. (2006). Model selection by normalized maximum likelihood. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 50, 167–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Myung, I. J., & Pitt, M. A. (1997). Applying Occam’s razor in modeling cognition: A Bayesian approach. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 79–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Navarro, D. J. (2004). A note on the applied use of MDL approximations. Neural Computation, 16, 1763–1768.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Newell, B. R., & Shanks, D. R. (2003). Take the best or look at the rest? Factors influencing “one-reason” decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 53–65.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Newell, B. R., Weston, N. J., & Shanks, D. R. (2003). Empirical tests of a fast-and-frugal heuristic: Not everyone “takes-the-best”. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 82–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Pachur, T., & Olsson, H. (2012). Type of learning task impacts performance and strategy selection in decision making. Cognitive Psychology, 65, 207–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Parkman, J. M. (1971). Temporal aspects of digit and letter inequality judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91, 191–205.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1988). Adaptive strategy selection in decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 534–552.Google Scholar
  62. Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Luce, M. F. (1996). When time is money: Decision behavior under opportunity-cost time pressure. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 66, 131–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Petrusic, W. M., & Jamieson, D. G. (1978). Relation between probability of preferential choice and time to choose changes with practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 471–482.Google Scholar
  65. Pitt, M. A., Myung, I. J., & Zhang, S. (2002). Toward a method of selecting among computational models of cognition. Psychological Review, 109, 472–491.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Platzer, C., & Bröder, A. (2012). Most people do not ignore salient invalid cues in memory-based decisions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 654–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Pohl, R. F., & Hilbig, B. E. (2012). The role of subjective linear orders in probabilistic inferences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 1178–1186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2008). The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Computation, 20, 873–922.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Ratcliff, R., & Smith, P. L. (2004). A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. Psychological Review, 111, 333–367.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Rieskamp, J., & Hoffrage, U. (2008). Inferences under time pressure: How opportunity costs affect strategy selection. Acta Psychologica, 127, 258–276.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Rieskamp, J., & Otto, P. E. (2006). SSL: A theory of how people learn to select strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 207–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Rissanen, J. J. (1996). Fisher information and stochastic complexity. IEEE Transactions on Information Theory, 42, 40–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Roberts, S., & Pashler, H. (2000). How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing. Psychological Review, 107, 358–367.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Roe, R. M., Busemeyer, J. R., & Townsend, J. T. (2001). Multialternative decision field theory: A dynamic connectionist model of decision making. Psychological Review, 108, 370–392.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Kuhberger, A., & Ranyard, R. (2011). The role of process data in the development and testing of process models of judgment and decision making. Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 733–739.Google Scholar
  76. Söllner, A., Bröder, A., & Hilbig, B. E. (2013). Deliberation versus automaticity in decision making: Which presentation format features facilitate automatic decision making? Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 278–298.Google Scholar
  77. Wasserman, L. (2000). Bayesian model selection and model averaging. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 44, 92–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53–85.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Wu, H., Myung, J. I., & Batchelder, W. H. (2010). On the minimum description length complexity of multinomial processing tree models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54, 291–303.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, School of Social SciencesUniversity of MannheimMannheimGermany

Personalised recommendations