Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 46, Issue 2, pp 181–190 | Cite as

The mere exposure effect for visual image

  • Kazuya Inoue
  • Yoshihiko Yagi
  • Nobuya Sato
Article

Abstract

Mere exposure effect refers to a phenomenon in which repeated stimuli are evaluated more positively than novel stimuli. We investigated whether this effect occurs for internally generated visual representations (i.e., visual images). In an exposure phase, a 5 × 5 dot array was presented, and a pair of dots corresponding to the neighboring vertices of an invisible polygon was sequentially flashed (in red), creating an invisible polygon. In Experiments 1, 2, and 4, participants visualized and memorized the shapes of invisible polygons based on different sequences of flashed dots, whereas in Experiment 3, participants only memorized positions of these dots. In a subsequent rating phase, participants visualized the shape of the invisible polygon from allocations of numerical characters on its vertices, and then rated their preference for invisible polygons (Experiments 1, 2, and 3). In contrast, in Experiment 4, participants rated the preference for visible polygons. Results showed that the mere exposure effect appeared only when participants visualized the shape of invisible polygons in both the exposure and rating phases (Experiments 1 and 2), suggesting that the mere exposure effect occurred for internalized visual images. This implies that the sensory inputs from repeated stimuli play a minor role in the mere exposure effect. Absence of the mere exposure effect in Experiment 4 suggests that the consistency of processing between exposure and rating phases plays an important role in the mere exposure effect.

Keywords

Mere exposure effect Visual image Perceptual fluency 

Notes

Author note

This work was supported by a Grant-in-aid (JSPS KAKENHI Grant Nos. 20530657, 15K00211, and 26870751) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science.

We would like to thank to Prof. Tadashi Kikuchi. He is an important collaborator on this study. However, we are saddened to report that he passed away prior to the completion of this article.

References

  1. Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968-1987. Psychological Bulletin, 106(2), 265–289. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.265 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1992). Stimulus recognition and the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 545–552. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.545 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1994). The Attribution and discounting of perceptual fluency: Preliminary tests of a perceptual fluency/attributional model of the mere exposure effect. Social Cognition, 12(2), 103–128. doi: https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1994.12.2.103 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brooks, J. O., & Watkins, M. J. (1989). Recognition memory and the mere exposure effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(5), 968–976. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.15.5.968 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral science (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Craver-Lemley, C., & Bornstein, R. F. (2006). Self-generated visual imagery alters the mere exposure effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(6), 1056–1060. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213925 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cumming, G. (2012). Understanding the new statistics: Effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  8. Elliott, R., & Dolan, R. J. (1998). Neural response during preference and memory judgments for subliminally presented stimuli: A functional neuroimaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(12), 4697–4704.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Franks, J. J., Bilbrey, C. W., Lien, K. G., & McNamara, T. P. (2000). Transfer-appropriate processing (TAP) and repetition priming. Memory & Cognition, 28(7), 1140–1151. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211815 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gordon, P. C., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Implicit learning and generalization of the “mere exposure” effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 492–500. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.492 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Jung, Y., & Chong, S. C. (2014). Effects of attention on visible and invisible adapters. Perception, 43(6), 549–568. doi: https://doi.org/10.1068/p7660 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Kass, R. E., & Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayes factors. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 90, 773–795. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kramer, R. S. S., & Parkinson, B. (2005). Generalization of mere exposure to faces viewed from different horizontal angles. Social Cognition, 23(2), 125–136. doi: https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.23.2.125.65625 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kunst-Wilson, W., & Zajonc, R. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207(4430), 557–558. doi: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7352271 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y., & Van Zandt, B. J. (1987). Nonspecific effects of exposure on stimuli that cannot be recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(4), 646–648. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.13.4.646 Google Scholar
  16. Matlin, M. W. (1971). Response competition, recognition, and affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19(3), 295–300. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031352 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Monahan, J. L., Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Subliminal mere exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects. Psychological Science, 11(6), 462–466. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00289 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Moreland, R. L., & Zajonc, R. (1977). Is stimulus recognition a necessary condition for the occurrence of exposure effects? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(4), 191–199. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.191 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 519–533. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver’s processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(4), 364–382. doi: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Rhodes, G., Halberstadt, J., & Brajkovich, G. (2001). Generalization of mere exposure effects to averaged composite faces. Social Cognition, 19(1), 57–70. doi: https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.19.1.57.18961 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Seamon, J. G., Brody, N., & Kauff, D. M. (1983). Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized: Effects of shadowing, masking, and cerebral laterality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9(3), 544–555. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.9.3.544 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Seamon, J. G., Ganor-Stern, D., Crowley, M. J., Wilson, S. M., Weber, W. J., O’Rourke, C. M., & Mahoney, J. K. (1997). A mere exposure effect for transformed three-dimensional objects: Effects of reflection, size, or color changes on affect and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25(3), 367–374. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211292 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Seamon, J. G., Williams, P. C., Crowley, M. J., Kim, I. J., Langer, S. A., Orne, P. J., & Wishengrad, D. L. (1995). The mere exposure effect is based on implicit memory: Effects of stimulus type, encoding conditions, and number of exposures on recognition and affect judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(3), 711–721. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.3.711 Google Scholar
  25. Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2009). The architecture of intuition: Fluency and affect determine intuitive judgments of semantic and visual coherence and judgments of grammaticality in artificial grammar learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(1), 39–63. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014678 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Vanderplas, J. M., & Garvin, E. A. (1959). The association value of random shapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 147–154. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048723 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Selker, R., … Morey, R. D. (2017). Bayesian inference for psychology: Part 2. Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7
  28. Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 989–1000. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.6.989 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Yagi, Y., Ikoma, S., & Kikuchi, T. (2009). Attentional modulation of the mere exposure effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 1403–1410. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017396 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Yang, S., Gallo, D. A., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Embodied memory judgements: A case of motor fluency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(5), 1359–1365. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016547 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(2, Pt. 2), 1–27. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025848 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inference. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151–175. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10(6), 224–228. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00154 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and TechnologyTsukubaJapan
  2. 2.Faculty of Engineering, Information, and SystemsUniversity of TsukubaTsukubaJapan
  3. 3.Kwansei Gakuin UniversityNishinomiyaJapan
  4. 4.Rissho UniversityShinagawaJapan

Personalised recommendations