Memory & Cognition

, Volume 43, Issue 2, pp 206–215 | Cite as

Memory for medication side effects in younger and older adults: The role of subjective and objective importance

  • Michael C. Friedman
  • Shannon McGillivray
  • Kou Murayama
  • Alan D. Castel
Article

Abstract

Older adults often experience memory impairments, but sometimes they can use selective processing and schematic support to remember important information. In the present experiments, we investigated the degrees to which younger and healthy older adults remembered medication side effects that were subjectively or objectively important to remember. Participants studied a list of common side effects and rated how negative these effects would be if they were to experience them, and they were then given a free recall test. In Experiment 1, the severity of the side effects ranged from mild (e.g., itching) to severe (e.g., stroke), and in Experiment 2, certain side effects were indicated as being critical to remember (i.e., “contact your doctor if you experience this”). We observed no age differences in terms of free recall of the side effects, and older adults remembered more severe side effects than mild effects. However, older adults were less likely to recognize the critical side effects on a later recognition test, relative to younger adults. These findings suggest that older adults can selectively remember medication side effects but have difficulty identifying familiar but potentially critical side effects, and this has implications for monitoring medication use in older age.

Keywords

Memory Aging Medications Side effects Older adults 

Notes

Author note

M.C.F. is now at the Harvard Initiative for Learning and Teaching, Harvard University. We thank Monika Holser, Stephanie Knipprath, and Angel Wu for help with data collection, and Barbara Knowlton, Douglas Bell, and Robert Bjork for helpful comments. This research was conducted in partial fulfillment of a doctoral dissertation by M.C.F. at the University of California, Los Angeles. This research was presented at the 15th Biennial Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta, GA. This research was supported in part by the National Institutes of Health (National Institute on Aging), Award Number R01AG044335.

References

  1. Arbuckle, T. Y., Cooney, R., Milne, J., & Melchior, A. (1994). Memory for spatial layouts in relation to age and schema typicality. Psychology and Aging, 9, 467–480.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bäckman, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Psychological compensation: A theoretical framework. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 259–283. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.259 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Castel, A. D. (2005). Memory for grocery prices in younger and older adults: The role of schematic support. Psychology and Aging, 20, 718–721.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Castel, A. D. (2007). Aging and memory for numerical information: The role of specificity and expertise in associative memory. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 62, 194–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Castel, A. D. (2008). The adaptive and strategic use of memory by older adults: Evaluative processing and value-directed remembering. In A. S. Benjamin & B. H. Ross (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 48, pp. 225–270). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(07)48006-9 Google Scholar
  8. Castel, A. D., Benjamin, A. S., Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (2002). The effects of aging on selectivity and control in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1078–1085. doi: 10.3758/BF03194325 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Castel, A. D., McGillivray, S., & Friedman, M. C. (2012). Metamemory and memory efficiency in older adults: Learning about the benefits of priority processing and value-directed remembering. In M. Naveh-Benjamin & N. Ohta (Eds.), Memory and aging: Current issues and future directions (pp. 245–270). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  10. Castel, A. D., Murayama, K., Friedman, M. C., McGillivray, S., & Link, I. (2013). Selecting valuable information to remember: Age-related differences and similarities in self-regulated learning. Psychology and Aging, 28, 232–242.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Chalfonte, B. L., & Johnson, M. K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 24, 403–416. doi: 10.3758/BF03200930 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cohen, R. L., Sandler, S. P., & Schroeder, K. (1987). Aging and memory for words and action events: Effects of item repetition and list length. Psychology and Aging, 2, 280–285.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Craik, F. I. M. (2002). Human memory and aging. In L. Bäckman & C. von Hofsten (Eds.), Psychology at the turn of the millennium (pp. 261–280). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  14. Craik, F. I. M., & Bosman, B. A. (1992). Age-related changes in memory and learning. In H. Bouma & J. A. M. Graafmans (Eds.), Gerontechnology (pp. 79–92). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: IOS Press.Google Scholar
  15. Craik, F. I. M., & McDowd, J. M. (1987). Age differences in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 474–479. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.474 Google Scholar
  16. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 717–726. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Friedman, M. C., & Castel, A. D. (2013). Memory, priority encoding, and overcoming high-value proactive interference in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 20, 660–683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Geraci, L., & Miller, T. M. (2013). Improving older adults’ memory performance using prior task success. Psychology and Aging, 28, 340.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Gutchess, A. H., Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Aging, self-referencing, and medial prefrontal cortex. Social Neuroscience, 2, 117–133.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). New York, NY: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60041-9 Google Scholar
  21. Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19, 27–39.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Hess, T. M. (2005). Memory and aging in context. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 383–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.383 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Hess, T. M., & Slaughter, S. J. (1990). Schematic knowledge influences on memory scene information in young and older adults. Developmental Psychology, 26, 855–865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hines, L. E., & Murphy, J. E. (2011). Potentially harmful drug–drug interactions in the elderly: A review. American Journal of Geriatric Pharmacotherapy, 9, 364–377.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jacoby, L. L., Bishara, A. J., Hessels, S., & Toth, J. P. (2005). Aging, subjective experience, and cognitive control: Dramatic false remembering by older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 131–148. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.131 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jacoby, L. L., Wahlheim, C. N., Rhodes, M. G., Daniels, K. A., & Rogers, C. S. (2010). Learning to diminish the effects of proactive interference: Reducing false memory for young and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 38, 820–829. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.6.820 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jones, T. C., & Jacoby, L. L. (2005). Conjunction errors in recognition memory: Modality-free errors for older adults but not for young adults. Acta Psychologica, 120, 55–73.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Kang, S. H. K., McDermott, K. B., & Cohen, S. M. (2008). The mnemonic advantage of processing fitness-relevant information. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1151–1156. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.6.1151 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Koutstaal, W., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 555–583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Liu, L. L., & Park, D. C. (2004). Aging and medical adherence: The use of automatic processes to achieve effortful things. Psychology and Aging, 19, 318–325. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.318 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a “new view” (pp. 145–162). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  33. Madan, C. R., & Spetch, M. L. (2012). Is the enhancement of memory due to reward driven by value or salience? Acta Psychologica, 139, 343–349.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14, 409–415.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospective memory components most at risk for older adults and implications for medication adherence. In D. C. Park & L. L. Liu (Eds.), Medical adherence and aging: Social and cognitive perspectives (pp. 49–75). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., & Jacoby, L. L. (2008). New considerations in aging and memory: The glass may be half full. In F. I. M. Craik & T. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 255–310). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  37. McGillivray, S., & Castel, A. D. (2011). Betting on memory leads to metacognitive improvement in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 26, 137–142.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. McGillivray, S., & Castel, A. D. (2014). Older and younger adults’ strategic control of metacognitive monitoring: The role of consequences, task experience, and prior knowledge. Manuscript under review.Google Scholar
  39. Miller, L. M. S. (2003). The effects of age and domain knowledge on text processing. Journals of Gerontology, 58B, P217–P223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Moore, A. A., Whiteman, E. J., & Ward, K. T. (2007). Risks of combined alcohol/medication use in older adults. American Journal of Geriatric Pharmacotherapy, 5, 64–74.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Morrow, D. G., Leirer, V. O., Altieri, P. A., & Fitzsimmons, C. (1994). When expertise reduces age differences in performance. Psychology and Aging, 9, 134–148.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Morrow, D. G., Menard, W. E., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Teller, T., & Bryant, D. (2001). The influence of expertise and task factors on age-differences in pilot communication. Psychology and Aging, 16, 31–46.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Murayama, K., Sakaki, M., Yan, V. X., & Smith, G. M. (2014). Type-1 error inflation in the traditional by-participant analysis to metamemory accuracy: A generalized mixed-effects model perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1287–1306.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult-age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1170–1187. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.6.1170 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Qato, D. M., Alexander, G. C., Conti, R. M., Johnson, M., Schumm, P., & Lindau, S. T. (2008). Use of prescription and over-the-counter medications and dietary supplements among older adults in the United States. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 300, 2867–2878.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Rhodes, M. G., Castel, A. D., & Jacoby, L. L. (2008). Associative recognition of face pairs by younger and older adults: The role of familiarity-based processing. Psychology and Aging, 23, 239–249.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). Whoops, I did it again: Commission errors in prospective memory. Psychology and Aging, 27, 46–53. doi: 10.1037/a0026112 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2007). The Dumbledore hypothesis of cognitive aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 295–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00524.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Umanath, S., & Marsh, E. J. (2014). Understanding how prior knowledge influences memory in older adults. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 408–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Webb, J., Davis, T. C., Bernadella, P., Clayman, M. L., Parker, R. M., Adler, D., & Wolf, M. S. (2008). Patient-centered approach for improving prescription drug warning labels. Patient Education and Counseling, 72, 443–449.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. White, B. P., Becker-Blease, K. A., & Grace-Bishop, K. (2006). Stimulant medication use, misuse, and abuse in an undergraduate and graduate student sample. Journal of American College Health, 54, 261–268.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Wolinsky, F. D., & Johnson, R. J. (1991). The use of health services by older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 46, S345–S357.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Zacks, R. T., & Hasher, L. (2006). Aging and long-term memory: Deficits are not inevitable. In E. Bialystok & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 162–177). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Zogg, J. B., Woods, S. P., Sauceda, J. A., Wiebe, J. S., & Simoni, J. M. (2012). The role of prospective memory in medication adherence: A review of an emerging literature. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 35, 47–62.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Michael C. Friedman
    • 1
  • Shannon McGillivray
    • 2
  • Kou Murayama
    • 3
    • 4
  • Alan D. Castel
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyWeber State UniversityOgdenUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ReadingReadingUK
  4. 4.Kochi University of TechnologyKamiJapan

Personalised recommendations