Advertisement

Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience

, Volume 14, Issue 4, pp 1454–1466 | Cite as

Affecting speed and accuracy in perception

  • Bruno R. BocanegraEmail author
Article

Abstract

An account of affective modulations in perceptual speed and accuracy (ASAP: Affecting Speed and Accuracy in Perception) is proposed and tested. This account assumes an emotion-induced inhibitory interaction between parallel channels in the visual system that modulates the onset latencies and response durations of visual signals. By trading off speed and accuracy between channels, this mechanism achieves (a) fast visuo-motor responding to course-grained information, and (b) accurate visuo-attentional selection of fine-grained information. ASAP gives a functional account of previously counterintuitive findings, and may be useful for explaining affective influences in both featural-level single-stimulus tasks and object-level multistimulus tasks.

Keywords

Emotion Vision Perception Attention Spatial frequency Localization Identification Speed Accuracy 

References

  1. Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 258–281. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arnell, K. M., Killman, K. V., & Fijavz, D. (2007). Blinded by emotion: Target misses follow attention capture by arousing distractors in RSVP. Emotion, 7, 465–477.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bocanegra, B. R., Huijding, J., & Zeelenberg, R. (2012). Beyond attentional bias: A perceptual bias in a dot-probe task. Emotion, 12, 1362–1366. doi: 10.1037/a0028415 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bocanegra, B. R., & Zeelenberg, R. (2009a). Dissociating emotion-induced blindness and hypervision. Emotion, 9, 865–873. doi: 10.1037/a0017749 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bocanegra, B. R., & Zeelenberg, R. (2009b). Emotion improves and impairs early vision. Psychological Science, 20, 707–713. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02354.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bocanegra, B. R., & Zeelenberg, R. (2011a). Emotion-induced trade-offs in spatiotemporal vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 272–282. doi: 10.1037/a0023188 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bocanegra, B. R., & Zeelenberg, R. (2011b). Emotional cues enhance the attentional effects on spatial and temporal resolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1071–1076. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0156-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Borst, G., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2010). Fear selectively modulates visual mental imagery and visual perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 833–839. doi: 10.1080/17470211003602420 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Breitmeyer, B. G., & Ogmen, H. (2000). Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: A comparison, review, and update. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1572–1595. doi: 10.3758/BF03212157 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Burr, D. C., Morrone, M. C., & Ross, J. (1994). Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements. Nature, 371, 511–513. doi: 10.1038/371511a0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Callaway, E. M. (1998). Local circuits in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 21, 47–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chun, M. M., & Potter, M. C. (1995). A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 109–127. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.1.109 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. De Jong, P. J., Koster, E. H., Van Wees, R., & Martens, S. (2009). Emotional facial expressions and the attentional blink: Attenuated blink for angry and happy faces irrespective of social anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 1640–1652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. De Valois, R. L., Albrecht, D. G., & Thorell, L. G. (1982). Spatial frequency selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex. Vision Research, 22, 545–559. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90113-4 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298, 1191–1194.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  18. Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2007). Masking disrupts reentrant processing in human visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1488–1497.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fox, E., Russo, R., & Georgiou, G. A. (2005). Anxiety modulates the degree of attentive resources required to process emotional faces. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 396–404. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.4.396 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Ben-Artzi, E., Jeczemien, P., Marom, S., & Hermesh, H. (2004). Depression impairs the ability to ignore the emotional aspects of facial expressions: Evidence from the Garner task. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 209–232.Google Scholar
  21. Holmes, A., Nielsen, M. K., & Green, S. (2008). Effects of anxiety on the processing of fearful and happy faces: An event-related potential study. Biological Psychology, 77, 159–173. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.10.003 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Itti, L., Koch, C., & Braun, J. (2000). Revisiting spatial vision: Towards a unifying model. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 17, 1899–1917.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kolassa, I. T., & Miltner, W. H. (2006). Psychophysiological correlates of face processing in social phobia. Brain Research, 1118, 130–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 571–579. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 209–235. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lee, T. H., Baek, J., Lu, Z.L., & Mather, M. (in press). How arousal modulates the visual contrast sensitivity function. Emotion.Google Scholar
  27. Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490. doi: 10.3758/BF03210951 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Maratos, F. A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2008). Identification of angry faces in the attentional blink. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 1340–1352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Maunsell, J. H., Ghose, G. M., Assad, J. A., McAdams, C. J., Boudreau, C. E., & Noerager, B. D. (1999). Visual response latencies of magnocellular and parvocellular LGN neurons in macaque monkeys. Visual Neuroscience, 16, 1–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Milders, M., Sahraie, A., Logan, S., & Donnellon, N. (2006). Awareness of faces is modulated by their emotional meaning. Emotion, 6, 10–17. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.10 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Most, S. B., & Wang, L. (2011). Dissociating spatial attention and awareness in emotion-induced blindness. Psychological Science, 22, 300–305.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Nicol, J. R., Perrotta, S., Caliciuri, S., & Wachowiak, M. P. (2013). Emotion-specific modulation of early visual perception. Cognition & Emotion, 27, 1478–1485. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.793654 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Öğmen, H., Purushothaman, G., & Breitmeyer, B. G. (2008). Metacontrast, target recovery, and the magno- and parvocellular systems: A reply to the perspective. Visual Neuroscience, 25, 611–616. doi: 10.1017/S0952523807080649 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Passamonti, L., Rowe, J. B., Ewbank, M., Hampshire, A., Keane, J., & Calder, A. J. (2008). Connectivity from the ventral anterior cingulate to the amygdala is modulated by appetitive motivation in response to facial signals of aggression. NeuroImage, 43, 562–570.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 773–783.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pessoa, L., Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Attentional control of the processing of neutral and emotional stimuli. Cognitive Brain Research, 15, 31–45.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pessoa, L., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2004). Neuroimaging studies of attention and the processing of emotion-laden stimuli. Progress in Brain Research, 144, 171–182. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)14412-3 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychological Science, 17, 292–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01701.x PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Roesch, E. B., Sander, D., Mumenthaler, C., Kerzel, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2010). Psychophysics of emotion: The QUEST for emotional attention. Journal of Vision, 10(3), 4:1–9. doi: 10.1167/10.3.4
  40. Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sagaspe, P., Schwartz, S., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Fear and stop: A role for the amygdala in motor inhibition by emotional signals. NeuroImage, 55, 1825–1835.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Schmidt, T., Niehaus, S., & Nagel, A. (2006). Primes and targets in rapid chases: Tracing sequential waves of motor activation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 1005–1016.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Smith, M. L., Cottrell, G. W., Gosselin, F., & Schyns, P. G. (2005). Transmitting and decoding facial expressions. Psychological Science, 16, 184–189.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Song, I., & Keil, A. (2013). Affective engagement and subsequent visual processing: Effects of contrast and spatial frequency. Emotion, 13, 748–757. doi: 10.1037/a0031553 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1994). “What” and “where” in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4, 157–165. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90066-3 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Van Dillen, L. F., Lakens, D., & den Bos, K. V. (2011). At face value: Categorization goals modulate vigilance for angry faces. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 235–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. van Honk, J., Tuiten, A., de Haan, E., van de Hout, M., & Stam, H. (2001). Attentional biases for angry faces: Relationships to trait anger and anxiety. Cognition & Emotion, 15, 279–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 624–631. doi: 10.1038/nn1057 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. West, G. L., Anderson, A. A. K., & Pratt, J. (2009). Motivationally significant stimuli show visual prior entry: Evidence for attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1032–1042. doi: 10.1037/a0014493 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Winston, J. S., Vuilleumier, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Effects of low-spatial frequency components of fearful faces on fusiform cortex activity. Current Biology, 13, 1824–1829.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Yeshurun, Y., & Carrasco, M. (2000). The locus of attentional effects in texture segmentation. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 622–627.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Yeshurun, Y., & Levy, L. (2003). Transient spatial attention degrades temporal resolution. Psychological Science, 14, 225–231. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02436 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Zwaan, R. A., & Pecher, D. (2012). Revisiting mental simulation in language comprehension: Six replication attempts. PLoS ONE, 7, e51382.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition, Institute of PsychologyLeiden UniversityLeidenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations