Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience

, Volume 13, Issue 4, pp 803–813 | Cite as

An unforgettable apple: Memory and attention for forbidden objects

Article

Abstract

Are we humans drawn to the forbidden? From jumbo-sized soft drinks to illicit substances, the influence of prohibited ownership on subsequent demand has made this question a pressing one. We know that objects that we ourselves own have a heightened psychological saliency, relative to comparable objects that are owned by others, but do these kinds of effects extend from self-owned to “forbidden” objects? To address this question, we developed a modified version of the Turk shopping paradigm in which “purchased” items were assigned to various recipients. Participants sorted everyday objects labeled as “self-owned”, “other-owned,” and either “forbidden to oneself” (Experiment 1) or “forbidden to everyone” (Experiment 2). Subsequent surprise recognition memory tests revealed that forbidden objects with high (Experiment 1) but not with low (Experiment 2) self-relevance were recognized as well as were self-owned objects, and better than other-owned objects. In a third and final experiment, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to determine whether self-owned and self-forbidden objects, which showed a common memory advantage, are in fact treated the same at a neurocognitive–affective level. We found that both object types were associated with enhanced cognitive analysis, relative to other-owned objects, as measured by the P300 ERP component. However, we also found that self-forbidden objects uniquely triggered an enhanced response preceding the P300, in an ERP component (the N2) that is sensitive to more rapid, affect-related processing. Our findings thus suggest that, whereas self-forbidden objects share a common cognitive signature with self-owned objects, they are unique in being identified more quickly at a neurocognitive level.

Keywords

Attention Memory Ownership Forbidden ERP Self-relevance 

Notes

Author note

G.T. is supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, and T.C.H. is supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. D.J.T. is supported by a grant from the European Research Council - 202893. We thank Nathan Wispinski, Lara Cooper, and Javier Granados-Samayoa for assistance with data collection.

References

  1. Balconi, M., & Pozzoli, U. (2012). Encoding of emotional facial expressions in direct and incidental tasks: An event-related potentials N200 effect. Journal of Neurotherapy, 16, 92–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beggan, J. K. (1992). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 229–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 139–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berlad, I., & Pratt, H. (1995). P300 in response to subject’s own name. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 96, 472–474.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bijvank, M. N., Konijn, E. A., Bushman, B. J., & Roelofsma, P. H. M. P. (2009). Age and violent-content labels make video games forbidden fruits for youth. Pediatrics, 123, 870–876. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0601 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bower, G. H., & Gilligan, S. G. (1979). Remembering information related to one’s self. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 420–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brehm, J. W., Stires, L. K., Sensenig, J., & Shaban, J. (1966). The attractiveness of an eliminated choice alternative. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 301–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological reactance. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
  9. Brenner, L., Rottenstreich, Y., Sood, S., & Bilgin, B. (2007). On the psychology of loss aversion: Possession, valence, and reversals of the endowment effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 369–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bushman, B. J. (1998). Effects of warning and information labels of full-fat, reduced-fat, and no-fat products. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 97–101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Chen, A., Weng, X., Yuan, J., Lei, X., Qui, J., Yao, D., & Li, H. (2008). The temporal features of self-referential processing evoked by Chinese handwriting. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 816–827.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chen, J., Yuan, J., Feng, T., Chen, A., Gu, B., & Li, H. (2011). Temporal features of the degree effect in self-relevance: Neural correlates. Biological Psychology, 87, 290–295.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cunningham, S. J., Turk, D. J., Macdonald, L. M., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Yours or mine? Ownership and memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 312–318. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.003 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cunningham, S. J., van den Bos, M., & Turk, D. J. (2011). Exploring the effects of ownership and choice on self-memory biases. Memory, 19, 449–461.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Folstein, J. R., & van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology, 45, 152–170.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Ganellen, R. J., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Why does self-reference promote incidental encoding? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 284–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gawronski, B., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Becker, A. P. (2007). I like it, because I like myself: Associative self-anchoring and post-decisional change of implicit evaluations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 221–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gray, H. M., Ambady, N., Lowenthal, W. T., & Deldin, P. (2004). P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 216–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gutz, L., Küpper, C., Renneberg, B., & Niedeggen, M. (2011). Processing social participation: An event-related brain potential study. NeuroReport, 22, 453–458.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Herbert, C., Pauli, P., & Herbert, B. M. (2011). Self-reference modulates the processing of emotional stimuli in the absence of explicit self-referential appraisal instructions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 653–661.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Huang, Y., Wang, L., & Shi, J. (2009). When do objects become more attractive? The individual and interactive effects of choice and ownership on object evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 713–722.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1990). Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase Theorem. Journal of Political Economy, 98, 1325–1348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kensinger, E. A. (2007). Negative emotion enhances memory accuracy: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 213–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Peyk, P., & Junghofer, M. (2007). Buzzwords: Early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychological Science, 18, 475–480.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Winkler, I., & Junghofer, M. (2009). Emotion and attention in visual word processing: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 80, 75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.004 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Klein, S. B. (2012). Self, memory, and the self-reference effect: An examination of conceptual and methodological issues. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 283–300.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Klein, S. B., & Loftus, E. M. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding: The contribution of elaborative and organisational processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 5–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Knetsch, J. L. (1989). The endowment effect and evidence of nonreversible indifference curves. American Economic Review, 79, 1277–1284.Google Scholar
  30. Ma, Q., Wang, K., Wang, X., Wang, C., & Wang, L. (2010). The influence of negative emotion on brand extension as reflected by the change of N2: A preliminary study. Neuroscience Letters, 485, 237–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Maddux, W. W., Yang, H., Falk, C., Adam, H., Adair, W., Endo, Y., & Heine, S. J. (2010). For whom is parting with possessions more painful? Cultural differences in the endowment effect. Psychological Science, 21, 1910–1917.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Morewedge, C. K., Shu, L. L., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2009). Bad riddance or good rubbish? Ownership and not loss aversion causes the endowment effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 947–951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ninomiya, H., Onitsuka, T., Chen, C. H., Sato, E., & Tashiro, N. (1998). P300 in response to the subject’s own face. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 52, 519–522.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31, 440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Otten, M., & Jonas, K. J. (2012). Out of the group, out of control? The brain responds to social exclusion with changes in cognitive control. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss071 Google Scholar
  36. Patel, S. H., & Assam, P. N. (2005). Characterization of N200 and P300: Selected studies of the event-related potential. International Journal of Medical Sciences, 2, 147–154.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O., & Echallier, J. F. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 184–187.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2128–2148.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Rogers, T., Kuiper, N., & Kirker, W. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 677–688.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Serbun, S. J., Shih, J. Y., & Gutchess, A. H. (2011). Memory for details with self-referencing. Memory, 19, 1004–1014.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Steinmetz, K. R. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2009). The effects of valence and arousal on the neural activity leading to subsequent memory. Psychophysiology, 46, 1190–1199. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00868.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–394. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.371 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Tacikowski, P., & Nowicka, A. (2010). Allocation of attention to self-name and self-face: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 84, 318–324.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Themanson, J. R., Khatcherian, S. M., Ball, A. B., & Rosen, P. J. (2012). An event-related examination of neural activity during social interactions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss058 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Turk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Brebner, J. L., Toma, A. S., Krigolson, O., & Handy, T. C. (2011a). When “it” becomes “mine”: Attentional biases triggered by object ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3725–3733.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Turk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Waiter, G. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2011b). Mine and me: Exploring the neural basis of object ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3657–3668.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. van den Bos, M., Cunningham, S. J., Conway, M. A., & Turk, D. J. (2010). Mine to remember: The impact of ownership on recollective experience. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1065–1071. doi: 10.1080/17470211003770938 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59–91.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Yuan, J., Meng, X., Yang, J., Yao, G., Hu, L., & Yuan, H. (2012). The valence strength of unpleasant emotion modulates brain processing of behavioral inhibitory control: Neural correlates. Biological Psychology, 89, 240–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Zhao, K., Wu, Q., Zimmer, H. D., & Fu, X. (2011). Electrophysiological correlates of visually processing subject’s own name. Neuroscience Letters, 491, 143–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada
  2. 2.University of BristolBristolUK

Personalised recommendations