Momentum-like effects and the dynamics of perception, cognition, and action
- 152 Downloads
Abstract
In a momentum-like effect, the likely future state of a current action or process is extrapolated. Momentum-like effects have been suggested to reflect dynamic processes, but such effects have not often been discussed in the broader literature on dynamic approaches to perception, cognition, and action. Several momentum-like effects are briefly described, and attempts to formulate dynamic theories of such effects are considered. Issues regarding dynamic representation that are relevant for theories of momentum-like effects (whether contingencies are invariant, stochastic, or arbitrary; bridging gaps between perception and action and between action and reinforcement; adaptiveness of such effects; influences of an observer’s knowledge, beliefs, and expectations; relationship of momentum-like effects to naïve physics and perception of causality) are discussed. Issues highlighted by a consideration of momentum-like effects relevant for dynamic approaches to other phenomena (multiple meanings and senses of “dynamic,” different meanings and connotations of “continuation” and “extrapolation,” perceptual inference of subjective or objective consequences, importance of time scale and temporal information, importance of the computational theory level, momentum-like effects as an example of predictive processing) are also discussed. Momentum-like effects provide examples of relatively simple dynamic processes that reveal and highlight issues relevant for study of dynamic approaches in a wide range of perceptual, cognitive, and action phenomena.
Keywords
Perception and action Momentum-Like effect Cognitive dynamics Predictive processing Anticipatory representationNotes
References
- Ashida, H. (2004). Action-specific extrapolation of target motion in human visual system. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1515-1524.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Bendixen, A., SanMiguel, I., & Schröger, E. (2012). Early electrophysiological indicators for predictive processing in audition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83, 120-131.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Blättler, C., Ferrari, V., Didierjean, A., & Marmèche, E. (2011). Representational momentum in aviation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1569-1577.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181-204.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Cohen, S. L., Riley, D. S., & Weigle, P. A. (1993). Tests of behavior momentum in simple and multiple schedules with rats and pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60(2), 255-291.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Cornelius, A. E., Silva, J. M., III, Conroy, D. E., & Petersen, G. (1997). The projected performance model: Relating cognitive and performance antecedents of psychological momentum. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 475-485.Google Scholar
- de-Wit, L., Machilsen, B., & Putzeys, T. (2010). Predictive coding and the neural response to predictable stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(26), 8702-8703.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Dube, W. V., Ahearn, W. H., Lionello-Denolf, K., & McIlvane, W. J. (2009). Behavioral momentum: Translational research in intellectual and developmental disabilities. The Behavior Analyst Today, 10, 238-253.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Finke, R. A., & Freyd, J. J. (1989). Mental extrapolation and cognitive penetrability: Reply to Ranney and proposals for evaluative criteria. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 403-408.Google Scholar
- Fodor, J. R. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
- Freyd, J. J. (1987). Dynamic mental representations. Psychological Review, 94, 427-438.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Freyd, J. J., & Finke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126-132.Google Scholar
- Freyd, J. J., & Jones, K. T. (1994). Representational momentum for a spiral path. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 968-976.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Friston, K. (2003). Learning and inference in the brain. Neural Networks, 16, 1325-1352.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Science, 360, 815-836.Google Scholar
- Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127-138.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Futterweit, L. R., & Beilin, H. (1994). Recognition memory for movement in photographs: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 163-179.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Gernigon, C., Briki, W., & Eykens, K. (2010). The dynamics of psychological momentum in sport: The role of ongoing history of performance patterns. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32, 377-400.Google Scholar
- Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 295-314.Google Scholar
- Graesser, A. C., & Nakamura, G. V. (1982). The impact of a schema on comprehension and memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 16, 59-109.Google Scholar
- Gray, R., & Thornton, I. M. (2001). Exploring the link between time to collision and representational momentum. Perception, 30, 1007-1022.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Grinblatt, M. & Han, B. (2005). Prospect theory, mental accounting, and momentum. Journal of Financial Economics, 78, 311-339.Google Scholar
- Haselton, M. G., Bryant, G. A., Wilke, A., Frederick, D. A., Galperin, A., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Moore, T. (2009). Adaptive rationality: An evolutionary perspective on cognitive bias. Social Cognition, 27, 733-763.Google Scholar
- Haselton, M. G., & Nettle, D. (2006). The paranoid optimist: An integrative model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 47-66.Google Scholar
- Hayes, A. E., & Freyd, J. J. (2002). Representational momentum when attention is divided. Visual Cognition, 9, 8-27.Google Scholar
- Hegarty, M. (2004). Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 280-285.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Helmholtz, H. (1860). Treatise on physiological optics, Volume 3. (J. P. C. Southhall, translation 1962). New York: Dover.Google Scholar
- Höffler, T. N., & Schwartz, R. N. (2011). Effects of pacing and cognitive style across dynamic and non-dynamic representations. Computers & Education, 57(2), 1716-1726.Google Scholar
- Hohwy, J. (2007). Functional integration and the mind. Synthese, 159, 315-328.Google Scholar
- Hosoya, T., Baccus, S. A., & Meister, M. (2005). Dynamic predictive coding by the retina. Nature, 436(7047), 71-77.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (1994). Judged displacement: A modular process? American Journal of Psychology, 107, 359-373.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (1995a). Cognitive representation of motion: Evidence for friction and gravity analogues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 241-254.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (1995b). Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 322-338.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (1996). Representational momentum, centripetal force, and curvilinear impetus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1049-1060.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (1997). Target size and displacement along the axis of implied gravitational attraction: Effects of implied weight and evidence of representational gravity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1484-1493.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2004). The perception of causality: Insights from Michotte’s launching effect, naive impetus theory, and representational momentum. In A. M. Oliveira, M. P. Teixeira, G. F. Borges, & M. J. Ferro (Eds.), Fechner Day 2004 (pp. 116-121). Coimbra: The International Society for Psychophysics.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 822-851.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2006a). Bridging the gap: Possible roles and contributions of representational momentum. Psicologica, 27, 1-34.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2006b). Computational theory and cognition in representational momentum and related types of displacement: A reply to Kerzel. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 174-177.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Approaches to representational momentum: Theories and models. In R. Nijhawan, & B. Khurana (Eds.), Space and time in perception and action (pp. 338-365). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2013a). Launching, entraining, and representational momentum: Evidence consistent with an impetus heuristic in perception of causality. Axiomathes, 23, 633-643.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2013b). Phenomenal causality I: Varieties and variables. Axiomathes, 23, 1-42.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2013c). Phenomenal causality II: Integration and implication. Axiomathes, 23, 485-524.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2014). Forms of momentum across space: Representational, operational, and attentional. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 1371-1403.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2015a). Forms of momentum across time: Behavioral and psychological. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 36, 47-82.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2015b). The varieties of momentum-like experience. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 1081-1119.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2017a). Momentum in music: Musical succession as physical motion. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 27, 14-30.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2017b). Toward a general theory of momentum-like effects. Behavioural Processes, 141, 50-66.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2018). Influences on representational momentum. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp. 121-138). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L. (2019). Representational gravity: Empirical findings and theoretical implications. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L., & Bharucha, J. J. (1988). Judged displacement in apparent vertical and horizontal motion. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 211-221.Google Scholar
- Hubbard, T. L., & Lange, M. (2010). Prior probabilities and representational momentum. Visual Cognition, 18, 1063-1087.Google Scholar
- Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Dotson, C. O. (2014). Psychological momentum: Why success breeds success. Review of General Psychology, 18, 19-33.Google Scholar
- Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Dotson, C. O. (2016). Psychological momentum -- A key to continued success. Frontiers in Psychology, 7,Article ID 1328.Google Scholar
- Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Mobily, K. (1980). “Psychological momentum”: A phenomenon and an empirical (unobtrusive) validation of its influence in a competitive sport tournament. Psychological Reports, 46, 391-401.Google Scholar
- Johnson, M. L., & Larson, S. (2003). “Something in the way she moves” - Metaphors of musical motion. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 63-84.Google Scholar
- Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Johnston, H. M., & Jones, M. R. (2006). Higher order pattern structure influences auditory representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 2-17.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83(5), 323-355.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological Review, 96(3), 459-491.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jordan, J. S. (2013). The wild ways of conscious will: What we do, how we do it, and why it has meaning. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 574.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Jordan, J. S., Cialdella, V., Schloesser, D S., & Bai, J. (2018). Forms of bias in cognitive science: Moving beyond perception, action, and cognition. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp. 350-365). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Jörges, B., & López-Moliner, J. (2017). Gravity as a strong prior: Implications for perception and action. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 11:203. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00203 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Juarrero, A. (2002). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Kaiser, M.K., Proffitt, D.R., Whelan, S.M. & Hecht, H. (1992). Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 669-690.Google Scholar
- Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 718-727.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kelly, M. H., & Freyd, J. J. (1987). Explorations of representational momentum. Cognitive Psychology, 19(3), 369-401.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
- Kerick, S. E., Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Hatfield, B. D. (2000). Psychological momentum in target shooting: Cortical, cognitive-affective, and behavioral responses. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 22, 1-20.Google Scholar
- Kerzel, D. (2002). A matter of design: No representational momentum without predictability. Visual Cognition, 9, 66-80.Google Scholar
- Kerzel, D., Jordan, J. S., & Müsseler, J. (2001). The role of perception in the mislocalization of the final position of a moving target. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 829-840.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Knops, A., Viarouge, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 803-821.Google Scholar
- Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). Impetus beliefs as default heuristics: Dissociation between explicit and implicit knowledge about motion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 439-453.Google Scholar
- Kubovy, M. (1983). Mental imagery majestically transforming cognitive psychology (Review of R. N. Shepard & L. Cooper, Mental Images and their Transformations). Contemporary Psychology, 28, 661-663.Google Scholar
- Kubovy, M., & Epstein, W. (2001). Internalization: A metaphor we can live without. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 618-625.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Larson, S. (2004). Musical forces and melodic expectations: Comparing computer models and experimental results. Music Perception, 21, 457-498.Google Scholar
- Larson, S. (2012). Musical forces: Motion, metaphor, and meaning in music. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
- Lee, T. S., & Mumford, D. (2003). Hierarchical Bayesian inference in the visual cortex. Journal of the Optical Society of America, A, Optics, Image Science & Vision, 20(7), 1434-1448.Google Scholar
- Lindemann, O., & Tira, M. D. (2011). Operational momentum in numerosity production judgments of multi-digit number problems. The Journal of Psychology, 219, 50-57.Google Scholar
- Mace, F. C., Lalli, J. S., Shea, M. C., Lalli, E. P., West, B. J., Roberts, M., & Nevin, J. A. (1990). The momentum of human behavior in a natural setting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 163-172.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Markman, K. D., & Guenther, C. L. (2007). Psychological momentum: Intuitive physics and naive beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 800-812.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Marr, D. (1982). Vision. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
- McBeath, M. K. (2018). Natural regularities and coupled predictive perceptual and cognitive biases: Why we evolved to systematically experience spatial illusions. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp. 276-294). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- McCloskey, M. (1983). Naive theories of motion. In D. Gentner & A. L. Stevens (Eds.). Mental models (pp. 299-324). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: Operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1324-1333.Google Scholar
- McCrink, K., & Hubbard, T. L. (2017). Dividing attention increases operational momentum. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 3, 230-245.Google Scholar
- McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 400-408.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Miller, G. (1962). Psychology: The Science of Mental Life. New York: Harper & Row Publishers.Google Scholar
- Munger, M. P., Dellinger, M. C., Lloyd, T. G., Johnson-Reid, K., Tonelli, N. J., Wolf, K., & Scott, J. M. (2006). Representational momentum in scenes: Learning spatial layout. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1557-1568.Google Scholar
- Munger, M. P., Solberg, J. L., & Horrocks, K. K. (1999). The relationship between mental rotation and representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1557-1568.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Neiworth, J. J., & Rilling, M. E. (1987). A method for studying imagery in animals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 13, 203-214.Google Scholar
- Neuhoff, J G. (2018). Adaptive biases in visual and auditory looming perception. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp. 180-190). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Nevin, J. A. (1988). Behavioral momentum and the partial reinforcement effect. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 44-56.Google Scholar
- Nevin, J. A. (1992). An integrative model for the study of behavioral momentum. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57, 301-316.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Nevin, J. A. (2012). Resistance to extinction and behavioral momentum. Behavioural Processes, 90, 89-97.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Nevin, J.A. (2015). Behavioral Momentum: A Scientific Metaphor. CreateSpaceIndependent Publishing, San Bernadino.Google Scholar
- Nevin, J. A., & Grace, R. C. (2000). Behavioral momentum and the law of effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 73-90.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Nevin, J. A., Mandell, C., & Atak, J. R. (1983). The analysis of behavioral momentum. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39, 49-59.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Nevin, J. A., & Shahan, T. A. (2011). Behavioral momentum theory: Equations and applications. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 877-895.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Nevin, J. A., Tota, M. E., Torquato, R. D., & Shull, R. L. (1990). Alternative reinforcement increases resistance to change: Pavlovian or operant contingencies? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 359-379.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Parry-Cruwys, D. E., Neal, C. M., Ahearn, W. H., Wheeler, E. E., Premchander, R., Loeb, M. B., & Dube, W. V. (2011). Resistance to disruption in a classroom setting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 363-367.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Philbeck, J. W., & Witt, J. K. (2015). Action-specific influences on perception and postperceptual processes: Present controversies and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 141(6), 1120-1144.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Pickering, M. J., & Clark, A. (2014). Getting ahead: Forward models and their place in cognitive architecture. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 451-456.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pinhas, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic. Cognition, 109, 408-415.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Podlesnik, C. A., & Shahan, T. A. (2009). Behavioral momentum and relapse of extinguished operant responding. Learning & Behavior, 37, 357-364.Google Scholar
- Podlesnik, C. A., & Shahan, T. A. (2010). Extinction, relapse, and behavioral momentum. Behavioural Processes, 84, 400-411.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Port, R. F., & van Gelder, T. (1995). Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Pratt, J., Spalek, T. M., & Bradshaw, F. (1999). The time to detect targets at inhibited and noninhibited locations: Preliminary evidence for attentional momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 730-746.Google Scholar
- Rao, R. P. N., & Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2(1), 79-87.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Reed, C. L., & Vinson, N. G. (1996). Conceptual effects on representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 839-850.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Roediger, H. L. III. (1996). Memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(2), 76-100.Google Scholar
- Ruppel, S. E., Fleming, C. N., & Hubbard, T. L. (2009). Representational momentum is not (totally) impervious to error feedback. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 49-58.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Schöner, G., Spencer, J. P., DFT Research Group. (2016) Dynamic thinking: A primer on dynamic field theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Shaw, J. M., Dzewaltowski, D. A., & McElroy, M. (1992). Self-efficacy and causal attributions as mediators of perceptions of psychological momentum. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 14, 134-147.Google Scholar
- Shepard, R. N. (1975). Form, formation, and transformation of internal representations. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 87–122). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- Shepard, R. N. (1981). Psychophysical complementarity. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (pp. 279–341). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
- Tabatabaeian, M. (2018). Beyond the here and now: Experimental studies in how action dynamics reflect complex cognitive processes (Order No. AAI10621530). Available from PsycINFO. (2070848997; 2018-26097-076). Retrieved from http://login.ezproxy1.lib.asu.edu/login?url=https://search-proquest-com.ezproxy1.lib.asu.edu/docview/2070848997?accountid=4485
- Taylor, J., & Demick, A. (1994). A multidimensional model of momentum in sports. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 6, 51-70.Google Scholar
- Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge:MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Vallerand, R. J., Colavecchio, P. G., & Pelletier, L. G. (1988). Psychological momentum and performance inferences: A preliminary test of the antecedents-consequences psychological momentum model. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 10, 92-108.Google Scholar
- van Kesteren, M. T. R., Rijpkema, M., Ruiter, D. J., & Fernández, G. (2013). Consolidation Differentially Modulates Schema Effects on Memory for Items and Associations. PLoS ONE 8(2): e56155. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0056155 PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Verfaillie, K., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1991). Representational momentum and event course anticipation in the perception of implied periodical motions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 302-313.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Weiss, Y., Simoncelli, E., & Adelson, E. H. (2002). Motion illusions as optimal percepts. Nature Neuroscience, 5(6), 598-604.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Witt, J. K. (2018). Spatial biases from action. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp. 307-323). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Witt, J. K., & Riley, M. A. (2014). Discovering your inner Gibson: Reconciling action-specific and ecological approaches to perception–action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(6), 1353-1370.Google Scholar
- Zago, M. (2018). Perceptual and motor biases in reference to gravity. In T. L. Hubbard (Ed.). Spatial biases in perception and cognition (pp. 156-166). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar