Advertisement

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

, Volume 81, Issue 2, pp 489–503 | Cite as

Which task will we choose first? Precrastination and cognitive load in task ordering

  • Lisa R. FournierEmail author
  • Emily Coder
  • Clark Kogan
  • Nisha Raghunath
  • Ezana Taddese
  • David A. Rosenbaum
Article

Abstract

Precrastination, as opposed to procrastination, is the tendency to embark on tasks as soon as possible, even at the expense of extra physical effort. We examined the generality of this recently discovered phenomenon by extending the methods used to study it, mainly to test the hypothesis that precrastination is motivated by cognitive load reduction. Our participants picked up two objects and brought them back together. Participants in Experiment 1 demonstrated precrastination by picking up the near object first, carrying it back to the farther object, and then returning with both. Also, participants given an additional cognitive task (memory load) had a higher probability of precrastinating than those not given the added cognitive task. The objects in Experiment 1 were buckets with balls that had a very low chance of spillage; carrying them required low demands on attention. The near-object-first preference was eliminated in Experiment 2, where the near and far objects were cups with water that had a high chance of spillage; carrying them required higher demands on attention. Had precrastination occurred in this case, it would have greatly increased cognitive effort. The results establish the generality of precrastination and suggest that it is sensitive to cognitive load. Our results complement others showing that people tend to structure their behavior to minimize cognitive effort. The main new discovery is that people expend more physical effort to do so. We discuss the applied implications of our findings, as well as the possibility that precrastination may be a default, automatic behavior.

Keywords

Precrastination Task ordering Decision making Cognitive load Dual task 

Notes

References

  1. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. New York: Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
  2. Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., & Pelz, J. B. (1995). Memory representations in natural tasks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1), 66-80.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.66.Google Scholar
  3. Ballard, D. H., Hayhoe, M. M., Pook, P. K., & Rao, R. P. (1997). Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(04), 723-742.Google Scholar
  4. Baroody, A. J., & Ginsburg, H. P. (1986). The relationship between initial meaningful and mechanical knowledge of arithmetic. In J. Hiebert (Ed.), Conceptual and procedural knowledge: The case of mathematics (pp. 75–112). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Bates, D., Achler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4, Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48.  https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 Google Scholar
  6. Botvinick, M. M., Buxbaum, L. J., Bylsma, L. M., & Jax, S. A. (2009a). Toward an integrated account of object and action selection: A computational analysis and empirical findings from reaching-to-grasp and tool-use. Neuropsychologia, 47(3), 671-683.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.024 Google Scholar
  7. Botvinick, M.M., Huffstetler, S., & McGuire, J.T. (2009b). Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(1), 16-27.  https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.9.1.16 Google Scholar
  8. Botvinick, M. M., & Rosen, Z. B. (2009). Anticipation of cognitive demand during decision-making. Psychological Research, 73(6), 835-842.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0197-8.Google Scholar
  9. Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment. 48, 306-307.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13 Google Scholar
  10. Camerer, C.F. & Hogarth, R.M. (1999). The effects of financial incentives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 7-42.Google Scholar
  11. Castiello, U. (1996). Grasping a fruit: Selection for action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(3), 582-603.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.22.3.582.Google Scholar
  12. Christenfeld, N. (1995). Choices from identical situations. Psychological Science, 6(1), 550-55.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00304.x.Google Scholar
  13. Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Umiltà, C. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Evidence for visuomotor priming effect. Neuroreport, 8(1), 347-349.Google Scholar
  14. Droll, J. A. & Hayhoe, M. M. (2007). Trade-offs between gaze and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1352-1365.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1352.Google Scholar
  15. Dunn, T. L., Lutes, D. J., & Risko, E. F. (2016). Metacognitive evaluation in the avoidance of demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1372-1388.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000236 Google Scholar
  16. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 286-290.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00382.x.Google Scholar
  17. Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Williford, C. L., Pagan, J. L., & Dismukes, R. K. (2003). Forgetting of intentions in demanding situations is rapid. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9(3): 147–162.  https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.9.3.147.Google Scholar
  18. Fournier, L. R., Stubblefield, A. M., Dyre, B. P., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2018). Starting or finishing sooner? Sequencing preferences in object transfer tasks. Psychological Research,  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1022-7
  19. Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48(1), 26-34.Google Scholar
  20. Grant, A. (2016). Why I Taught Myself to Procrastinate. New York Times Sunday Review. http://www.nytimes.com/2016/01/17/opinion/sunday/why-i-taught-myself-to-procrastinate.html?emc=eta1&_r=0
  21. Haxby, J. V., Petit, L., Ungerleider, L. G., & Courtney, S. M. (2000). Distinguishing the functional roles of multiple regions in distributed neural systems for visual working memory. Neuroimage, 11(2), 145-156.Google Scholar
  22. Hewitt, P.L., & Flett, G.L. (1990). Perfectionism and depression: A multidimensional analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 5, 423-438.Google Scholar
  23. Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (2001). Detection by action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-defined templates in search. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1), 84-88.  https://doi.org/10.1038/82940.Google Scholar
  24. Jax, S. A., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2010). Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object. Cognition, 115(2), 350-355.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.01.004.Google Scholar
  25. Jax, S. A., & Rosenbaum D. A. (2007). Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: Evidence that the dorsal stream does not only control visually guided actions in real time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 425–441.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.425.Google Scholar
  26. Kool, W., McGuire, J.T., Rosen, Z.B., & Botvinick, M.M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 665-682.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020198.Google Scholar
  27. Lewandowsky, S. (2014). 300 million years of pre-crastination. http://www.psychonomic.org/featured-content-detail/300-million-years-of-precrastination
  28. McDaniel, M.A., Einstein, G.O., Stout, A.C., & Morgan, Z. (2003). Aging and maintaining intentions over delays: Do it or lose it. Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 823-835.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.823 Google Scholar
  29. Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768-774.  https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1.Google Scholar
  30. Pavese, A., & Buxbaum, L. J. (2002). Action matters: The role of action plans and object affordances in selection for action. Visual Cognition, 9(4-5), 559-590.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280143000584.Google Scholar
  31. R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/
  32. Richtel, M. (2014). Sometimes, early birds are too early. New York Times, page BU3. http://www.nytimes.com/2014/07/20/business/sometimes-early-birds-are-too-early.html?src=twr&_r=1.
  33. Rosch, E. (1999). Principles of categorization. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), Concepts: Core readings (pp. 189-206). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  34. Rosenbaum, D. A., Chapman, K. M., Weigelt, M., & Weiss, D. J. (2012). Cognition, action, and object manipulation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 138, 924-946.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027839 Google Scholar
  35. Rosenbaum, D. A., Gong, L., & Potts, C. A. (2014). Pre-crastination: Hastening sub-goal completion at the expense of extra physical effort. Psychological Science, 25(7), 1487-1496.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614532657.Google Scholar
  36. Ruiz Fernández, S., Leonhard, T., Lachmair, M., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Processing order in dual-tasks when the duration of motor responses varies [Special issue: Cognitive science]. Universitas Psychologica, 12(5), 1439-1452.  https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.UPSY12-5.podt Google Scholar
  37. Ruiz Fernández, S., Leonhard, T., Rolke, B., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Processing two tasks with varying task order: Central stage duration influences central processing order. Acta Psychologica, 137(1), 10–17.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.01.016 Google Scholar
  38. Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 830-846.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.830.Google Scholar
  39. Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.Google Scholar
  40. van der Wel, R. P. Fleckenstein, R., Jax, S., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2007). Hand path priming in manual obstacle avoidance: Evidence for abstract spatio-temporal forms in human motor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1117-1126.Google Scholar
  41. Wasserman, E.A. (2018). Precrastination: The fierce urgency of now. Learning & Behavior.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-018-0358-6.
  42. Wasserman, E. A., & Brzykcy, S. J. (2015). Pre-crastination in the pigeon. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(4), 1130–1134.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0758-3.Google Scholar
  43. Zbrodoff, N. J. (1999). Effects of counting in alphabet arithmetic: Opportunistic stopping and priming of intermediate steps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(2), 299-317.Google Scholar
  44. Zeigarnik B. (1927). Das Behalten erledigter und unerledigter Handlungen. Psychologische Forschung, 9, 1-85.Google Scholar
  45. Zhu, M., Yang, Y., Hsee, C. K., Johar, G., & Lee, L. (2018). The Mere Urgency Effect. Journal of Consumer Research.  https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucy008.

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lisa R. Fournier
    • 1
    Email author
  • Emily Coder
    • 1
  • Clark Kogan
    • 2
  • Nisha Raghunath
    • 1
  • Ezana Taddese
    • 1
  • David A. Rosenbaum
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyWashington State UniversityPullmanUSA
  2. 2.Center for Interdisciplinary Statistical Education and ResearchWashington State UniversityPullmanUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of California RiversideRiversideUSA

Personalised recommendations