Advertisement

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

, Volume 81, Issue 2, pp 359–376 | Cite as

Learned and cued distractor rejection for multiple features in visual search

  • Brad T. Stilwell
  • Shaun P. VeceraEmail author
Article

Abstract

Ignoring distracting information is critical for effective visual search. When individuals are cued to ignore a stimulus, they first attend the to-be-ignored stimulus before learning to reject it. Individuals can learn to overcome the initial distraction produced by the explicit cues, although this cued distractor rejection appears for only one distractor feature. Multiple distractor colors cannot be rejected effectively, even with extensive experience. We asked if this apparent limit on distractor rejection was caused by a restriction on the number of different features (i.e., colors) that could be learned and rejected as distractors. To explore this potential capacity limitation, we asked if attention can learn to reject the smallest possible number of multiple distractors, namely, two. In four experiments examining cued distractor rejection, individuals searched through heterogeneously colored arrays containing reliable, non-target color information. In Experiments 1 and 2, we explicitly cued individuals with which of two colors (both colors in Experiment 1 or one color in Experiment 2) could be safely ignored. Cued distractors were not reliably rejected, replicating previous findings. Additionally, in Experiment 2, we presented a to-be-ignored color without explicit cues and we found that these “uncued” distractors were reliably rejected. In Experiments 3 and 4, we presented the to-be-ignored color information without explicit cues; individuals learned to reliably ignore multiple distractor colors without explicit cueing. These results suggest that learned distractor rejection is better suited to experience-driven learning than explicitly cued distractor learning: Explicit cueing reliably interferes with learned distractor rejection.

Keywords

Visual search Attention: Selective Attention 

Notes

References

  1. Arita, J. T., Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2012). Templates for rejection: Configuring attention to ignore task-irrelevant features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 580–584.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027885 Google Scholar
  2. Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 485–496.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205306 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beck, V. M., & Hollingworth, A. (2015). Evidence for negative feature guidance in visual search is explained by spatial recoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(5), 1190–1196.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000109 Google Scholar
  4. Beck, V. M., & Hollingworth, A. (2017). Competition in saccade target selection reveals attentional guidance by simultaneously active working memory representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(2), 225–230.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000306 Google Scholar
  5. Beck, V. M., Hollingworth, A., & Luck, S. J. (2012). Simultaneous control of attention by multiple working memory representations. Psychological Science, 23(8), 887–898.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612439068 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beck, V. M., Luck, S. J., & Hollingworth, A. (2018). Whatever you do, don’t look at the: Evaluating guidance by an exclusionary attentional template. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(4), 645–662.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000485 Google Scholar
  7. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision  https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357
  8. Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychology Review, 97(4), 523–547.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.523 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 28–71.  https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0681 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (Vol. 2nd).  https://doi.org/10.1234/12345678
  11. Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2013). Context-dependent control over attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(3), 836–848.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030027 Google Scholar
  12. Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology  https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.01.1.p042
  13. Cunningham, C. A., & Egeth, H. E. (2016). Taming the White Bear. Psychological Science, 27(4), 476–485.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615626564 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2015). Direct evidence for active suppression of salient-but-irrelevant sensory inputs. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1740–1750.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615597913 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2017). Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(1), 45–62.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2017). Distinguishing among potential mechanisms of singleton suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000484
  18. Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018). The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 79–92.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Irons, J. L., Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2012). All set! Evidence of simultaneous attentional control settings for multiple target colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 758–775. ://doi.org/10.1037/a0026578 Google Scholar
  20. Kugler, G., ’T Hart, B. M., Kohlbecher, S., Einhäuser, W., & Schneider, E. (2015). Gaze in visual search is guided more efficiently by positive cues than by negative cues. PLoS ONE, 10(12), e0145910.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145910 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kunar, M. A., Flusberg, S., Horowitz, T. S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Does contextual cueing guide the deployment of attention? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 816–828.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.816 Google Scholar
  22. Moher, J., & Egeth, H. E. (2012). The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(8), 1590–1605.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-012-0358-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Moher, J., Lakshmanan, B. M., Egeth, H. E., & Ewen, J. B. (2014). Inhibition drives early feature-based attention. Psychological Science, 25(2), 315–324.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613511257 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Moore, K. S., & Weissman, D. H. (2010). Involuntary transfer of a top-down attentional set into the focus of attention: Evidence from a contingent attentional capture paradigm. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(6), 1495–1509.  https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.6.1495 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology  https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061
  26. Olivers, C. N. L., Peters, J., Houtkamp, R., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2011). Different states in visual working memory: When it guides attention and when it does not. Trends in Cognitive Sciences  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.004
  27. Roper, Z. J. J., & Vecera, S. P. (2012). Searching for two things at once: Establishment of multiple attentional control settings on a trial-by-trial basis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(6), 1114–1121.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0297-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Stilwell, B. T., & Vecera, S. P. (2017, November). The Interaction Between Explicitly Cued and Experience-Dependent Distractor Rejection on Visual Attention. Poster presented at the 58th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society. Vancouver, BC.Google Scholar
  29. Treisman, A. (1998). Feature binding, attention and object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 353(1373), 1295–1306.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1998.0284 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Tsal, Y., & Makovski, T. (2006). The attentional white bear phenomenon: the mandatory allocation of attention to expected distractor locations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 351–363.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.351 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Vatterott, D. B., Mozer, M. C., & Vecera, S. P. (2018). Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 80(2), 485–499.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1465-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Vatterott, D. B., & Vecera, S. P. (2012). Experience-dependent attentional tuning of distractor rejection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(5), 871–878.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0280-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5–13.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0. A revised model of visual search. Psychnomic Bulletin & Review, 1(2), 202–238.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200774 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Woodman, G. F., Carlisle, N. B., & Reinhart, R. M. G. (2013). Where do we store the memory representations that guide attention? Journal of Vision, 13(3), 1–1.  https://doi.org/10.1167/13.3.1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 363–377.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.363 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Zehetleitner, M., Goschy, H., & Müller, H. J. (2012). Top-down control of attention: It’s gradual, practice-dependent, and hierarchically organized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(4), 941–957.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027629 Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychological and Brain SciencesUniversity of IowaIowa CityUSA

Personalised recommendations