Advertisement

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

, Volume 80, Issue 5, pp 1072–1082 | Cite as

The effect of sadness on global-local processing

  • Adrian von Mühlenen
  • Lauren Bellaera
  • Amrendra Singh
  • Narayanan Srinivasan
Registered Reports and Replications

Abstract

Gable and Harmon-Jones (Psychological Science, 21(2), 211-215, 2010) reported that sadness broadens attention in a global-local letter task. This finding provided the key test for their motivational intensity account, which states that the level of spatial processing is not determined by emotional valence, but by motivational intensity. However, their finding is at odds with several other studies, showing no effect, or even a narrowing effect of sadness on attention. This paper reports two attempts to replicate the broadening effect of sadness on attention. Both experiments used a global-local letter task, but differed in terms of emotion induction: Experiment 1 used the same pictures as Gable and Harmon-Jones, taken from the IAPS dataset; Experiment 2 used a sad video underlaid with sad music. Results showed a sadness-specific global advantage in the error rates, but not in the reaction times. The same null results were also found in a South-Asian sample in both experiments, showing that effects on global/local processing were not influenced by a culturally related processing bias.

Keywords

Sadness Global-local processing Emotion induction 

References

  1. Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2(3), 249-255.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bellaera, L., & von Muhlenen, A. (2017). The effect of induced sadness and moderate depression on attention networks. Cognition and Emotion, 31(6), 1140–1152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bellaera, L., von Mühlenen, A., & Watson, D. G. (2014). When being narrow minded is a good thing: Locally biased people show stronger contextual cueing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(6), 1242–1248.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 45-62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boduroglu, A., Shah, P., and Nisbett, R. E. (2009). Cultural differences in allocation of attention in visual information processing. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 40, 349–360.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49-59.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bradley, M. M. & Lang, P. J. (2007). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary & G. Bernston (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd, pp. 581-607). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 5–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chepenik, L. G., Cornew, L. A., & Farah, M. J. (2007). The influence of sad mood on cognition. Emotion, 7(4), 802-811.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Clore, G. L., & Palmer, J. (2009). Affective guidance of intelligent agents: How emotion controls cognition. Cognitive Systems Research, 10(1), 21-30.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C., & Isbell, L. (2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (pp. 27-62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  12. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  13. Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. M. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167–196). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Domachowska, I., Heitmann, C., Deutsch, R., Goschke, T., Scherbaum, S., & Bolte, A. (2016). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention: Registered replication report of Gable and Harmon-Jones (2008). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 50-56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Duncan, S., & Barrett, L. F. (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: A neurobiological analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 1184-1211.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183-201.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175-191.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Fenske, M. J., & Eastwood, J. D. (2003). Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing emotion: evidence from flanker tasks. Emotion, 3(4), 327-343.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Finucane, A. M., Whiteman, M. C., & Power, M. J. (2010). The effect of happiness and sadness on alerting, orienting, and executive attention. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13(6), 629-639.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313-332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19(5), 476-482.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21(2), 211-215.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2013). Does arousal per se account for the influence of appetitive stimuli on attentional scope and the late positive potential? Psychophysiology, 50(4), 344-350.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2016). Assessing the Motivational Dimensional Model of emotion–cognition interaction: Comment on Domachowska, Heitmann, Deutsch, et al., (2016). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 57–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34-40.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Huntsinger, J. R. (2012). Does positive affect broaden and negative affect narrow attentional scope? A new answer to an old question. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 595-600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Huntsinger, J. R., Clore, G. L., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2010). Mood and global-local focus: Priming a local focus reverses the link between mood and global-local processing. Emotion, 10(5), 722-726.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Izard, C. E., Bartlett, E. S., & Marshall, A. G. (1972). Patterns of emotions. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  29. Izard, C. E., Dougherty, F. E., Bloxom, B. M., & Kotsch, N. E. (1974). The Differential Emotions Scale: A method of measuring the meaning of subjective experience of discrete emotions. Nashville: Vanderbilt University, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
  30. JASP Team (2016). JASP (Version 0.8.0.0) [Computer software].Google Scholar
  31. Kimchi, R., & Palmer, S. E. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(4), 521-535.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Kringelbach, M. & Phillips, H. (2014). Emotion: Pleasure and Pain in the Brain. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
  33. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2005). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.Google Scholar
  34. LeBel, E. P., Vanpaemel, W., McCarthy, R. J., Earp, B. D., & Elson, M. (2017). A Unified Framework to Quantify the Trustworthiness of Empirical Research. Retrieved from psyarxiv.com/uwmr8
  35. Masuda T., & Nisbett R. E. (2001) Attending holistically vs. analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 922–934.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Melcher, T., Obst, K., Mann, A., Paulus, C., & Gruber, O. (2012). Antagonistic modulatory influences of negative affect on cognitive control: Reduced and enhanced interference resolution capability after the induction of fear and sadness. Acta Psychologica, 139(3), 507-514.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Mottus, R., Allik, J., Realo, A., Rossier, J., Zecca, G., Ah-Kion, J., … Johnson, W. (2012). The Effect of Response Style on Self-Reported Conscientiousness Across 20 Countries. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(11), 1423–1436.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Nisbett, R.E., & Miyamoto, Y. (2005). The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 467–473.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Philippot, P. (1993). Inducing and assessing differentiated emotion-feeling states in the laboratory. Cognition & Emotion, 7(2), 171-193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. D., & Gross, J. (2007) Emotion elicitation using films. In J. A. Coan & J. J. Allen (Eds.), Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 9-28), New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(1), 383-388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Schaefer, A., Nils, F., Sanchez, X., & Philippot, P. (2010). Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: A new tool for emotion researchers. Cognition & Emotion, 24(7), 1153–1172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Schwartz, G. E., & Weinberger, D. A. (1980). Patterns of emotional responses to affective situations: Relations among happiness, sadness, anger, fear, depression, and anxiety. Motivation and Emotion, 4, 175-191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Srinivasan, N., & Gupta, R. (2010). Emotion-attention interactions in recognition memory for distractor faces. Emotion, 10(2), 207-215.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Srinivasan, N., & Hanif, A. (2010). Global-happy and local-sad: Perceptual processing affects emotion identification. Cognition and Emotion, 24(6), 1062-1069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Tan, H. K., Jones, G. V., & Watson, D. G. (2009). Encouraging the perceptual underdog: Positive affective priming of nonpreferred local–global processes. Emotion, 9(2), 238-247.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). The stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Uddenberg, S., & Shim, W. M. (2015). Seeing the world through target-tinted glasses: Positive mood broadens perceptual tuning. Emotion, 15(3), 319–328.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2007). A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 779–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder J.N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2011). Statistical Evidence in Experimental Psychology: An Empirical Comparison Using 855 t Tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 291–298.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Adrian von Mühlenen
    • 1
  • Lauren Bellaera
    • 2
  • Amrendra Singh
    • 3
  • Narayanan Srinivasan
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WarwickCoventryUK
  2. 2.Faculty of EducationUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK
  3. 3.Centre of Behavioural and Cognitive SciencesUniversity of AllahabadAllahabadIndia

Personalised recommendations