Advertisement

The uphill battle for action-specific perception

  • Emily L. Laitin
  • Michael J. Tymoski
  • Nathan L. Tenhundfeld
  • Jessica K. WittEmail author
Article
  • 38 Downloads

Abstract

The action-specific account of perception states that a perceiver’s ability to act influences the perception of the environment. For example, participants tend to perceive distances as farther when presented up hills than on the flat ground. This tendency is known as the distance-on-hill effect. However, there is debate as to whether these types of effects are truly perceptual. Critics of the action-specific account of perception claim that the effects could be due to participants guessing the hypothesis and trying to comply with the experimental demands. The present study aims to explore the distance-on-hill effect and determine whether it is truly perceptual or whether past results were due to response bias. Participants judged the relative distance to targets on a hill and the flat ground. We found the distance-on-hill effect in virtual reality using a visual matching task. The distance-on-hill effect persisted even when participants were given explicit feedback about their estimates. We also found that the effect went away, as predicted by a perceptual explanation, when participants had to match the distance between two cones that were both on hills. These results offer important steps toward the painstaking task of determining whether action’s effect on perception is truly perceptual.

Keywords

Perception and action Scene Perception Embodied perception 

Notes

Author note

Data, scripts, and supplementary materials available at https://osf.io/ua6vn/. This work was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation (BCS-1632222 and BCS-1348916 to J.K.W.).

References

  1. Aarts, A. A., Anderson, J. E., Anderson, C. J., Attridge, P. R., Attwood, A., Axt, J., ... Zuni, K. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251).  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716
  2. Armbrüster, C., Wolter, M., Kuhlen, T., Spijkers, W., & Fimm, B. (2008). Depth perception in virtual reality: Distance estimations in peri- and extrapersonal space. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 11(1), 9–15.  https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2007.9935 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using {lme4}. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(4), 1076–1096.Google Scholar
  5. Durgin, F. H., Baird, J. A., Greenburg, M., Russell, R., Shaughnessy, K., & Waymouth, S. (2009). Who is being deceived? The experimental demands of wearing a backpack. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 964–969.  https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.5.964 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Eves, F. F., Thorpe, S. K. S., Lewis, A., & Taylor-Covill, G. A. H. (2014). Does perceived steepness deter stair climbing when an alternative is available? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 637–644.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0535-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fajen, B. R., & Phillips, F. (2012). Spatial perception and action. In D. A. Waller & L. Nadel (Eds.), Handbook of spatial cognition (Ch. 3). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  8. Firestone, C. (2013). How “paternalistic” is spatial perception? Why wearing a heavy backpack doesn’t—and couldn’t—make hills appear steeper. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 455–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2014). “Top-down” effects where none should be found. Psychological Science, 25(1), 38–46.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613485092 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for “top-down” effects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000965
  11. Foley, J. M. (1977). Effect of distance information and range on two indices of visually perceived fdistance. Perception, 6, 449–460.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p060449 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gilbert, D. T., King, G., Pettigrew, S., & Wilson, T. D. (2016). Comment on “Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science” Science, 351(6277).  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad7243
  13. Gray, R., Navia, J. A., & Allsop, J. (2014). Action-specific effects in aviation: What determines judged runway size? Perception, 43, 145–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 1166–1186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Howe, C. Q., & Purves, D. (2005). Natural-scene geometry predicts the perception of angles and line orientation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(4), 1228–1233.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0409311102 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. King, Z. R., Tenhundfeld, N. L., & Witt, J. K. (2017). What you see and what you are told: An action-specific effect that is unaffected by explicit feedback. Psychological Research, 82(3), 507–519.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0848-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26.  https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lessard, D. A., Linkenauger, S. A., & Proffitt, D. R. (2009). Look before you leap: Jumping ability distance perception. Perception, 38(12), 1863–1866.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p6509 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Loomis, J. M. (2016). Proposed applications of research on action-specific effects are premature. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(1), 77–79.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.12.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Loomis, J. M., Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 906–921.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.906 Google Scholar
  21. Philbeck, J. W., & Loomis, J. M. (1997). Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 72–85.Google Scholar
  22. Philbeck, J. W., & Witt, J. K. (2015). Action-specific influences on perception and postperceptual processes: Present controversies and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 141(6), 1120–1144.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Distance perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 131–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14(2), 106–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016.Google Scholar
  26. Stefanucci, J., Proffitt, D., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2005). Distances appear different on hills. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(6), 1052–1060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Stefanucci, J. K., & Geuss, M. N. (2009). Big people, little world: The body influences size perception. Perception, 38(12), 1782–1795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Sugovic, M., Turk, P., & Witt, J. K. (2016). Perceived distance and obesity : It ’s what you weigh , not what you think. Acta Psychologica, 165, 1–8.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.01.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Tenhundfeld, N. L., & Witt, J. K. (2017). Distances on hills look farther than distances on flat ground: Evidence from converging measures. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(4), 1165–1181.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1305-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Thompson, W. B., Willemsen, P., Gooch, A. A., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (2004). Does the quality of the computer graphics matter when judging distances in visually immersive environments? Presence Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 13(5), 560–571.  https://doi.org/10.1162/1054746042545292 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Wesp, R., Cichello, P., Gracia, E. B., & Davis, K. (2004). Observing and engaging in purposeful actions with objects influences estimates of their size. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(8), 1261–1267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Wesp, R., & Gasper, J. (2012). Is size misperception of targets simply justification for poor performance? Perception, 41(8), 994–996.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p7281 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Wilmer, J. B. (2008). How to use individual differences to isolate functional organization, biology, and utility of visual functions; with illustrative proposals for stereopsis. Spatial Vision, 21(6), 561–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Witt, J. K. (2011). Action’s effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 201–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Witt, J. K. (2017). Action potential influences spatial perception: Evidence for genuine top-down effects on perception. Psychonomic Bullitin & Review, 24(4), 999–1021.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1184-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., Augustyn, J. S., Cook, A., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). The long road of pain: Chronic pain increases perceived distance. Experimental Brain Research, 192(1), 145–148.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1594-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2005). See the ball, hit the ball: Apparent ball size is correlated with batting average. Psychological Science, 16(12), 937–938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2010). Performance and ease influence perceived speed. Perception, 39(10), 1341–1353.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p6699 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Witt, J. K., Sugovic, M., & Dodd, M. D. (2016). Action-specific perception of speed is independent of attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(3), 880–890.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-1047-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Witt, J. K., Sugovic, M., & Taylor, J. E. T. (2012). Action-specific effects in a social context: Others’ abilities influence perceived speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 715–725.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026261 Google Scholar
  41. Witt, J. K., Sugovic, M., Tenhundfeld, N. L., & King, Z. (2016). An action-specific effect on perception that avoids all pitfalls. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Witt, J. K., Tenhundfeld, N. L., & Tymoski, M. J. (2017). Is there a chastity belt on perception? Psychological Science. Advance online publication.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617730892
  43. Woods, A. J., Philbeck, J. W., & Danoff, J. V. (2009). The various perceptions of distance: An alternative view of how effort affects distance judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1104–1117.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013622 Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyColorado State UniversityFort CollinsUSA
  2. 2.Warfighter Effectiveness Research CenterUnited States Air Force AcademyUSAF AcademyUSA

Personalised recommendations