Gibson (1950) argued that the ground surface is uniquely important for spatial perception because we are terrestrial beings who depend on the ground surface for locomotion. Consistent with this view, research with adult participants has indicated that the ground surface plays a dominant role in the perception of spatial layout (e.g., Feria, Braunstein, & Andersen, 2003; He, Wu, Ooi, Yarbrough, & Wu, 2004; Meng & Sedgwick, 2001, 2002; Sinai, Ooi, & He, 1998; Wu, He, & Ooi, 2008). For example, Sinai et al. showed that the presence of discontinuities on a ground surface strongly affected distance perception and visually directed action.

If the ground surface is of special importance among the surfaces of our environment, it should be superior to ceiling surfaces and other planes in perceptual tasks. Indeed, several studies have provided evidence that visual search is made more efficient by a ground-like instead of a ceiling-like arrangement of the search array (McCarley & He, 2000, 2001; see also Morita & Kumada, 2003). Moreover, a ground surface facilitates 3-D size estimation (Champion & Warren, 2010) and detection of change (Bian & Andersen, 2010) more than ceilings and other surfaces do, and visual space is perceived as being less compressed when viewing a ground surface than when viewing a ceiling surface (Bian & Andersen, 2011).

The ground surface also plays a dominant role in the perception of relative distances. Bian, Braunstein, and Andersen (2005) showed adult participants two rods surrounded by ground and ceiling surfaces. One of the rods covered more ground texture than ceiling texture, and the other covered more ceiling texture. As a result, the two surfaces provided contradictory information about the rods’ distances. For example, in Fig. 1, which shows a display similar to those used by Bian et al. (2005), the ground surface specifies that the left rod/toy is near, and the ceiling specifies that it is far away. In contrast, the ground surface specifies the right rod/toy as being far away, and the ceiling specifies that it is near. Bian et al. (2005) found that the rods’ perceived relative distances were determined by the ground surface. The participants judged the rod that covered more ground texture to be nearer, disregarding the depth information from the ceiling. This ground dominance effect was independent of differences in the rods’ heights. Moreover, Bian, Braunstein, and Andersen (2006) found that the ground dominance effect was independent of location in the visual field: It occurred when the ground and ceiling surfaces were both positioned above the fixation point, when both were below the fixation point, and when one was above and the other below the fixation point. In a developmental study, Bian and Andersen (2008) showed that younger as well as older adults responded to the ground dominance effect. However, the effect was weaker in the older participants.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Example of an experimental display. The ground and ceiling textures provide contradictory information about the relative distances of the two toys. The background texture was a rectangular picture, and the toys were rubber toys attached to rods. The rubber toy–rod combinations were held in place by magnets

It has remained unknown whether the ground surface dominates spatial perception early in life as well as in adulthood. The ground dominance effect may be a bias that, through evolution, has become a part of our perceptual system (e.g., McCarley & He, 2000), since most of our motor acts rely on the ground plane (Gibson, 1950). If this is true, even infants might be subject to the effect. In the present study, we investigated the effects of ground versus ceiling surface texture on the perception of relative distance in 5- and 7-month-old infants. We used a preferential reaching method similar to that used previously in studies of infants’ responsiveness to the distance information provided by surface texture.

Several studies have demonstrated that infants respond to objects’ relative distances specified by texture cues (Arterberry, 2008; Arterberry, Yonas, & Bensen, 1989; Hemker, Granrud, Yonas, & Kavšek, 2010; Hemker & Kavšek, 2010; Yonas, Elieff, & Arterberry, 2002; Yonas, Granrud, Arterberry, & Hanson, 1986). For example, Yonas et al. (2002) showed 5- and 7-month-old infants two equidistant toys that were attached to a depiction of a textured ground surface that appeared to recede in depth. One toy was positioned near the lower edge of the surface, and the other was near the upper edge. The lower toy appeared to be within reach, and the upper one beyond reach. Yonas et al. (2002) found that individual infants began to display reliable reaching preferences for the apparently nearer toy between 5 and 6 1/2 months of age. With a similar display, Hemker et al. (2010) found preferential reaching in 5- and 7-month-old infants. However, the reaching preference was stronger among the older than among the younger infants. In an additional condition, the ground texture was omitted from the display, making the differential relative heights of the toys the sole pictorial depth cue. Again, the 7-month-old infants reached preferentially for the apparently nearer toy, whereas the 5-month-old infants did not respond to the distance information specified by relative height in isolation. The 7-month-olds’ reaching preference was significantly stronger in the texture-cues-plus-relative-height condition than in the relative-height-as-the-sole-depth-cue condition. These findings suggest that infants 5 and 7 months of age respond to the distance information provided by texture cues and that relative height is a weaker source of information for distance. Hemker and Kavšek (2010) complemented this research by showing that infants are more sensitive to the linear perspective of the texture’s surface contours than to the texture gradients of compression, perspective, and density.

The present study extended these earlier studies and examined whether infants display a ground dominance effect in perceiving objects’ relative distances. Five- and 7-month-old infants were shown a display in which toys were affixed to rods that made contact with a textured ground and a textured ceiling (Fig. 1). To maximize the effectiveness of the distance information provided by surface texture, highly regular textures adapted from Hemker et al. (2010) were used. The rods’ positions were manipulated so that the ground and ceiling provided contradictory information about the rods’/toys’ relative distances. To minimize the impact of relative height, the toys were positioned at the same height.

The infants viewed the display under monocular (one eye patched) and binocular conditions, and their reaches toward the two toys were observed. Only reaches for the toys were analyzed; reaches to other parts of the display were not recorded. On the basis of previous findings that infants tend to reach toward the nearer of two objects, and that infants respond to depth illusions created by pictorial depth cues under monocular but not binocular viewing conditions (e.g., Yonas & Granrud, 1985), we expected the infants to reach preferentially toward the toy that they perceived as being nearer in the monocular condition. The binocular condition served as a control for the possibility that infants might reach more often for one of the toys for a reason other than a perceived depth difference. If the infants’ reaching behavior was determined by two-dimensional features within the display, no difference in reaching should be found between the viewing conditions, because these features were present in both conditions. If infants exhibited a stronger reaching preference for the apparently nearer toy (as specified by the ground surface) under monocular than under binocular conditions, this would provide evidence that they responded to the relative distance specified by texture on the ground in the monocular condition and to binocular information specifying that the toys were equidistant in the binocular condition.

Method

Participants

A group of 21 5-month-old infants (10 females, 11 males; mean age = 153 days, range = 148–160 days) and another of 27 7-month-old infants (13 females, 14 males; mean age = 214 days, range = 209–221 days) participated in the experiment. In the 5-month-old group, 30 additional infants were not included in the final sample because of too few reaches (n = 7), fussiness (n = 18), or reaching for only one side in all trials of one or both (monocular and binocular) viewing conditions (n = 5). In the 7-month group, 24 additional infants were excluded because of too few reaches (n = 3), fussiness (n = 17), reaching for only one side in all trials of one or both viewing conditions (n = 3), or experimenter error (n = 1).

The participants were recruited by letter and follow-up telephone calls. The names of the infants were obtained from birth records provided by the municipal authorities of the city of Bonn, Germany. Data protection was guaranteed. The infants’ parents were given toy animals. After the study, they were provided with information about the results of this research. All participants were treated in accord with the ethical guidelines of the American Psychological Association and the Society for Research in Child Development, and the study was approved by the ethics committee of the Institute of Psychology at the University of Bonn.

Apparatus

The display was a 119 cm wide × 27 cm high (134.42° × 56.74° at a viewing distance of 25 cm) rectangular picture. The display depicted two black-and-white, regularly spaced textured surfaces—a ground and a ceiling surface—that appeared to recede in depth (Fig. 1). The display was fixed on a white 170 × 120 cm planar surface. Two black side panels (185 × 95 cm), which were oriented at a 100º angle from the experimental display, blocked the room and the experimenters from the infants’ view. An orange-colored curtain 13.5 cm from the display could be lowered to cover it. Two cameras recorded the infants’ reaching and looking behavior. One camera (model Hercules Classic Silver) was mounted above the display. It was directed downward and videotaped the infants’ reaches. The other camera (model Logitech Webcam C905) was positioned in the middle of the display. This camera was used to observe the infant’s looking and reaching behavior. The lens of the camera was black and was nearly indistinguishable from the black middle area of the display. The room was illuminated such that there were no reflections or shadows on the experimental display.

Stimuli

The textured surfaces consisted of white trapezoids on a black background. The ground surface was 11 cm high. The trapezoids in the lowest row of the ground surface were 1.8 cm away from the bottom edge of the display and 2.5 cm high. The upper row of trapezoids had a height of 0.4 cm. The stripes of black background were 1 cm high at the bottom of the ground surface and 0.16 cm high at the top of the surface. The trapezoids’ bottom contours were 3.2 cm wide at the bottom of the ground surface and 0.8 cm wide at the top. The black background stripes were 2 cm wide at the bottom and 0.4 cm wide at the top of the ground surface. The ceiling surface was constructed by rotating the ground surface by 180°. The distance between the ground and the ceiling surface was 5 cm.

Two identical rubber toys attached to rods were affixed to the display (see Fig. 1), held in place by magnets. Each target (rubber toy–rod combination) was placed 7 cm in horizontal distance from the center of the display. Three pairs of targets were used: two orange penguins attached to orange rods, two green and yellow frogs attached to yellow rods, and two blue monkeys attached two blue rods. The rods’ colors were similar to the rubber toys’ colors in order to accentuate their coherence. When affixed to the display, the targets induced strong depth impressions. After the experiment, the parents were asked to describe the display. They consistently reported that they perceived the depth effect. None indicated that they perceived the rods as being slanted in depth. Furthermore, none reported that they perceived the upper target as being elevated above the ground. We additionally asked several adults whether they perceived the rods as slanted in depth or elevated above the ground. None answered either question in the affirmative. The rubber toys measured 4 × 4 × 4 cm; the rods measured 3 × 14.5 cm. One rubber toy of each target pair was attached 10.25 cm above the lower edge of the rod, and the other rubber toy was attached 4.25 cm above the lower edge of the rod. When affixed to the display, the rubber toys were on the same horizontal axis. They were positioned in the black gap between the ground and the ceiling surfaces. One rod covered more texture of the ground than of the ceiling surface, and the other rod covered more texture of the ceiling than of the ground surface.

Procedure

Before testing began, the experimenters explained the procedure to the parents. The parents then read an information sheet and signed a consent form. The infants were given the opportunity to play with replicas of the experimental targets. The toys in these replicas were attached to the middle of the rods. The infants were given the targets in a random order for approximately 15 s each. The goal of this warm-up phase was to make the infants familiar with the unity of the rubber toys and the rods.

The parent was then seated on a chair with wheels that moved on a runner. The parent could easily roll the chair toward the display. The infant was seated on the parent’s lap. Parents were instructed to hold their children around the waist and to keep them positioned in the middle of the two toys. This point was marked on the curtain, so that the infant’s position could be adjusted. Other than monitoring the child’s position, parents were asked not to interact with their children during the experiment. The study’s hypotheses were explained after completion of the experimental session.

At the beginning of a trial, the infant was 45 cm from the display. If the infant did not orient to the display, a bell positioned behind the display was rung for approximately 5 s to attract the infant’s attention. The parent was then instructed to roll toward the display. At the nearest point, the infant was approximately 25 cm from the display. The shoulder height of the infants was at the height of the rubber toys, such that both toys were equally easy to reach. When the infant had reached for one of the toys or when 30 s had elapsed without a reach occurring, the parent was instructed to roll away from the display. Then, the next trial was initiated.

At the beginning of the experiment and after every third trial, the display was covered by the curtain. One experimenter then changed the targets (penguins vs. monkeys vs. frogs) and their positions (lower rod on the left vs. lower rod on the right side). The orders of presentation of the targets were the same in both viewing conditions. The targets’ initial position was randomly assigned to the participants.

The infants viewed the display in two blocks of trials: monocular and binocular. In the monocular condition, one eye was carefully occluded by a hypo-allergenic adhesive eye patch. It was randomly determined which eye was covered. Each viewing condition had a maximum of 18 trials. After the infant reached this maximum in the first condition, the infant took a 3- to 5-min break before the second condition started. Additional breaks were taken whenever the infant appeared to be tired or inattentive or if the infant did not reach for the rubber toys in several successive trials. If the infant was too fussy to continue, the experimental session was terminated. The first condition was stopped and was followed by the second condition after 18 trials or if it was necessary to take a break, provided that the infant had reached for one of the toys at least six times. Only infants who made at least six directed reaches in each condition were included in the final sample.

The order of the viewing conditions was counterbalanced across the sample. In the 5-month-old sample, 10 infants started with the monocular condition and 11 with the binocular condition. In the 7-month-old sample, 14 infants started with the monocular and 13 with the binocular condition.

Criteria for scoring infants’ reaches

Two experimenters observed the infant’s looking and reaching behavior on a computer monitor attached to the two cameras that filmed the infant. The experimenters independently scored the direction of the infant’s reaches by pressing buttons attached to a computer. The interrater agreement was very good (see, e.g., Landis & Koch, 1977), κ = .87 (Cohen, 1960). If the two experimenters had scored a trial differently, they jointly coded the trial again from videotape.

Criteria for directed reaching

Directed reaching for one of the two toys was scored if the infant looked at the toy before reaching for it. In the case of an infant reaching for both toys simultaneously, a directed reach was scored for the toy that was fixated. Reaching for the rods or any other contact with the display was scored as “without direction.” Reaches “without direction” and trials in which no reach occurred were excluded from the data analysis. Only directed reaches were analyzed.

Results

Directed reaches were divided into reaches to the toy whose rod covered a greater proportion of the ground surface (“lower” target) and reaches to the toy whose rod covered a greater proportion of the ceiling surface (“upper” target). The percentage of reaches to the lower target was computed for both the monocular and binocular viewing conditions by dividing the number of reaches to the lower target by the total sum of reaches to the lower and upper targets. The percentages of reaches to the lower target were statistically analyzed. If the percentage of reaches to the lower target was significantly higher under monocular than under binocular viewing conditions, this would indicate that the infants responded to the pictorial depth cues in the experimental display.

A preliminary data analysis found no effects of viewing condition order (monocular vs. binocular trials first) and initial target position (lower target on the left vs. the right). The data from these groups were therefore combined for subsequent analyses. Sex interacted significantly (α = .05) with age and viewing condition (monocular vs. binocular), F(1, 44) = 11.16, p = .002, \( \eta_{\mathrm{P}}^2=.20 \). The 5-month-old female infants exhibited a significantly higher percentage of reaches to the lower target in the monocular (M = 69.81, SD = 11.56) than in the binocular (M = 49.31, SD = 13.16) viewing condition, paired t(9) = 4.05, two-tailed p = .003, effect size d = 1.28. This effect was smaller and nonsignificant in the 7-month-old females (monocular, M = 61.65, SD = 16.67; binocular, M = 56.64, SD = 12.55). In the male sample, the 7-month-olds reached significantly more often for the lower target under monocular than under binocular conditions (monocular, M = 61.78, SD = 14.90; binocular, M = 41.95, SD = 10.33), paired t(13) = 3.68, two-tailed p = .003, d = 0.98. The 5-month-olds, however, exhibited no monocular–binocular difference (monocular, M = 57.14, SD = 19.14; binocular, M = 60.61, SD = 8.61). These sex differences are difficult to explain and have not been observed in prior research on infant pictorial depth perception. They may be an artifact of having small samples when the participants are divided into groups of boys and girls. We therefore collapsed the data across both sexes.

Table 1 presents the mean numbers of directed reaches and the mean percentages of reaches to the lower target. Percentages of reaches to the lower target were analyzed in a 2 × 2 mixed ANOVA with age (5 vs. 7 months) as a between-subjects factor and viewing condition (monocular vs. binocular) as a within-subjects factor.

Table 1 Mean number of directed reaches and mean percentage of reaches to the lower target in each viewing condition

The analysis revealed a significant main effect for viewing condition, F(1, 46) = 10.54, p = .002, η P 2 = .19. The main effect of age did not reach statistical significance. Moreover, no significant interaction between viewing condition and age emerged. The main effect for viewing condition indicated a higher percentage of reaches to the lower target in the monocular (mean percentage = 62.36, SD = 15.95) than in the binocular (M = 51.74, SD = 13.17) viewing condition.

A planned paired t test was calculated for each age group to further investigate the effect of viewing condition on the percentages of reaches to the lower target. For the 5-month-old infants, the effect of viewing condition was significant in a one-tailed test, t(20) = 1.75, p = .048, d = 0.38 (see Table 1). For the 7-month-olds, the effect of viewing condition was highly significant in a two-tailed t test, t(26) = 2.91, p = .007, d = 0.56 (see Table 1).

In sum, both age groups exhibited a stronger reaching preference for the lower target under monocular viewing than under binocular viewing. Planned analyses showed that this effect was slightly stronger in the older than in the younger participants. The overall ANOVA, however, did not reveal a substantial age difference: The effect of viewing condition was not modulated significantly by age.

In addition, planned one-sample t tests were conducted, which compared the percentages of reaches to the lower target in each viewing condition to chance (50 %). The infants reached to the lower target significantly more often than 50 % of the time in the monocular condition, t(47) = 5.37, p ≤ .001 (two-tailed), d = 0.78, but not in the binocular condition, t(47) = 0.91, p = .37. Similarly, analyses for each age group separately showed that both the 5-month-olds, t(20) = 3.58, p = .002, d = 0.78, and the 7-month-olds, t(26) = 3.94, p = .001, d = 0.76, reached significantly more than 50 % of the time for the lower target in the monocular viewing condition. In the binocular condition, the percentage of reaches to the lower target was marginally greater than 50 % for the 5-month-olds, t(20) = 1.97, p = .063, and did not differ from chance for the 7-month-olds, t(26) = –0.38, p = .71.

Participant loss was high, which is typical in preferential reaching studies (e.g., Corrow, Granrud, Mathison, & Yonas, 2011, 2012; Yonas & Granrud, 2006). This was probably due, in part, to our strict inclusion criteria. For example, all reaches in which the infants did not look at the toy for which they were reaching were excluded from the data analysis (see also Hemker et al., 2010). However, Slaughter and Suddendorf (2007), in their review on participant loss in infant vision studies, found no correlation between exclusion rates and experimental outcomes. Furthermore, we conducted an analysis of the participants who produced data in both the monocular and binocular viewing conditions, but who were excluded for not satisfying the selection criteria, so as to examine whether their reaching behavior was consistent with the general findings. Table 2 summarizes the mean percentages of reaches to the lower target in each viewing condition for these participants. The excluded 5- and 7-month-old infants reached more often for the lower target in the monocular than in the binocular condition. They therefore behaved similarly to the infants who were included in the sample, and their exclusion did not affect the results of the study.

Table 2 Mean percentage of reaches to the lower target in each viewing condition by infants excluded from the sample

Discussion

Under monocular viewing conditions, the 5- and 7-month-old infants reached more often for a toy that was specified as being nearer by the ground surface than for a toy that was specified as being nearer by the ceiling surface. These infants therefore exhibited a ground dominance effect: They attended to the distance information provided by the ground surface more than to information provided by the ceiling. The results indicate that the ground plays a dominant role in distance perception and visually guided reaching at a very early age, before infants can crawl and walk. The finding of a weaker reaching preference for the lower target in the binocular condition indicates that the infants’ monocular reaching preference cannot be explained as a response to two-dimensional display features such as the toys’ positions relative to the tops of the rods. Moreover, the weaker reaching preference under binocular viewing is inconsistent with the assumption that the ground dominance effect is a general bias to attend to the lower part of the visual field. If so, the infants would have shown similar reaching preferences for the lower target under monocular and binocular viewing conditions. The results, therefore, indicate that the infants’ monocular reaching behavior was based on the information provided by surface texture that specified a difference in the distances of the two toys.

The overall data analyses indicated that the 5- and the 7-month-old infants displayed the ground dominance effect and sensitivity to the distance information provided by surface texture. Although no statistically significant age differences were found, the effect was somewhat weaker in the 5-month-old than in the 7-month-old infants. This finding is consistent with the preferential reaching results found by Hemker et al. (2010), who demonstrated sensitivity to the depth information provided by a textured ground surface in 5- and 7-month-old infants, as well as a stronger response to texture cues in the older age group.

An alternative explanation for this study’s results might be that the infants responded to the differential relative heights of the targets and not to their perceived relative distances. This interpretation, however, is unlikely for several reasons. First, Bian, Braunstein, and Andersen (2005, 2006) did not find an effect of differences in the rods’ heights on adults’ distance judgments. Consistent with this observation, several studies have found that relative height is a weak depth cue for adults (e.g., Rock, Shallo, & Schwartz, 1978) as well as for children (e.g., McGurk & Jahoda, 1974). Furthermore, we observed reaching for toys positioned at the same height, and the height differences between the rods was smaller than that in the Hemker et al. (2010) study. That study employed a display very similar to that used in the present study, and Hemker et al. established a reliable effect of texture cues in infants 5 and 7 months of age: Relative height in isolation had either no (5-month-olds) or a significantly smaller (7-month-olds) effect on infants’ reaching (but see Arterberry, 2008). Nevertheless, further research will be needed to further examine the impact of relative height information.

The findings from several studies have indicated that the infant visual system uses heuristic assumptions to disambiguate two-dimensional retinal image cues to extract information about three-dimensional object shape and spatial layout. For example, Imura, Tomonaga, Yamaguchi, and Yagi (2008) and Shirai, Kanazawa, and Yamaguchi (2005) tested infants’ sensitivity to shading and found that 4- and 5-month-old infants behaved as if they made use of the assumption that objects are usually lit from above. More recently, Corrow et al. (2011) established that 6-month-old infants use the assumption that faces are convex, and Corrow et al. (2012) found that 5- and 7-month-olds employ an assumption that objects in general are convex in perceiving three-dimensional object shape. The present findings suggest that the ground dominance effect, the assumption that objects’ distances are primarily specified by their positions on a ground surface, is a basic constraint that is also applied by the infants’ visual system. This is consistent with the assumption that our visual system has adapted to the depth information provided by ground texture because our locomotion is usually confined to the ground surface (e.g., Gibson, 1950; Nakayama, He, & Shimojo, 1995). As a consequence, the ground dominance effect may be inherent in our visual system from birth onward. Alternatively, our visual system may be tuned to quickly learn to extract depth information from ground texture during the first months of life.

Our experiment replicated and extended numerous preferential reaching studies that had observed that 5- and 7-month-old infants are sensitive to pictorial depth cues, such as surface texture (e.g., Hemker et al., 2010; Yonas et al., 1986; for a review, see Kavšek, Granrud, & Yonas, 2009). It suggests that the ground surface is superior to the ceiling surface in the perception of distance and the planning of directed reaching from 5 months onward.

It remains unknown, however, whether responsiveness to depth from texture and the ground dominance effect are present earlier in life. Studies using looking methods have found responsiveness to several pictorial depth cues in infants as young as 3 months (e.g., Bertin & Bhatt, 2006; Tsuruhara et al., 2010; for a review, see Kavšek, Yonas, & Granrud, 2012). Future research will be needed to determine whether the ground dominance effect can be found in infants 3 months of age or younger.