Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 15, Issue 6, pp 1035–1053 | Cite as

Semantic processing in “associative” false memory

  • C. J. Brainerd
  • Y. Yang
  • V. F. Reyna
  • M. L. Howe
  • B. A. Mills
Theoretical and Review Articles

Abstract

We studied the semantic properties of a class of illusions, of which the Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm is the most prominent example, in which subjects falsely remember words that are associates of studied words. We analyzed DRM materials for 16 dimensions of semantic content and assessed the ability of these dimensions to predict interlist variability in false memory. For the more general class of illusions, we analyzed pairs of presented and unpresented words that varied in associative strength for the presence of these same 16 semantic properties. DRM materials proved to be exceptionally rich in meaning, as indexed by these semantic properties. Variability in false recall, false recognition, and backward associative strength loaded on a single semantic factor (familiarity/meaningfulness), whereas variability in true recall loaded on a quite different factor (imagery/concreteness). For word association generally, 15 semantic properties varied reliably with forward or backward association between words. Implications for semantic versus associative processing in this class of illusions, for dual-process theories, and for semantic properties of word associations are discussed. nt]mis|The present article was supported, in part, by grants from the National Institutes of Health (MH-061211) and the National Science Foundation (BCS 0553225) to C.J.B. and V.F.R., a grant from the National Cancer Institute (R13CA126359) to V.F.R., and a grant from the Economic and Social Research Council U.K. (RES-062-23-0452) to M.L.H.

Keywords

False Alarm Rate False Memory Semantic Processing False Recognition Semantic Property 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anisfeld, M., & Knapp, M. (1968). Association, synonymity, and directionality in false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 171–179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Armitage, P., Berry, G., & Matthews, J. N. S. (2002). Statistical methods in medical research (4th ed.). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barnhardt, T. M., Choi, H., Gerkens, D. R., & Smith, S. M. (2006). Output position and word relatedness effects in a DRM paradigm: Support for a dual-retrieval process theory of free recall and false memories. Journal of Memory & Language, 55, 213–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bjorklund, D. F., & Muir, J. E. (1988). Children’s development of free recall memory: Remembering on their own. Annals of Child Development, 5, 79–123.Google Scholar
  6. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. C-1). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology.Google Scholar
  7. Brainerd, C. J., Forrest, T. J., Karibian, D., & Reyna, V. F. (2006). Development of the false-memory illusion. Developmental Psychology, 42, 962–979.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Brainerd, C. J., Payne, D. G., Wright, R., & Reyna, V. F. (2003). Phantom recall. Journal of Memory & Language, 48, 445–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2005). The science of false memory. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Ceci, S. J. (2008). Developmental reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 343–382.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Mojardin, A. H. (1999). Conjoint recognition. Psychological Review, 106, 160–179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Wright, R., & Mojardin, A. H. (2003). Recollection rejection: False-memory editing in children and adults. Psychological Review, 110, 762–784.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Brainerd, C. J., & Wright, R. (2005). Forward associative strength, backward associative strength, and the false memory illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31, 554–567.Google Scholar
  14. Brainerd, C. J., Wright, R., Reyna, V. F., & Payne, D. G. (2002). Dual-retrieval processes in free and associative recall. Journal of Memory & Language, 46, 120–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Budson, A. E., Todman, R. W., Chong, H., Adams, E. H., Kensinger, E. A., Krangel, T. S., & Wright, C. I. (2006). False recognition of emotional word lists in aging and Alzheimer disease. Cognitive & Behavioral Neurology, 19, 71–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cann, D. R., McRae, K., & Katz, A. N. (2006, November). Knowledge types underlying false recall in the Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Paper presented at the 47th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Houston.Google Scholar
  17. Carneiro, P., Fernandez, A., & Dias, A. R. (2008). The influence of theme identifiability on false memories: Evidence for age-dependent opposite effects. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  18. Corson, Y., & Verrier, N. (2007). Emotion and false memories: Valence or arousal? Psychological Science, 18, 208–211.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Cotel, S. C., Gallo, D. A., & Seamon, J. G. (2008). Nonconscious activation causes false memories: Experimental control of conscious processes in the Deese, Roediger, and McDermott task. Consciousness & Cognition, 17, 210–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dannenbring, G. L., & Briand, K. (1982). Semantic priming and the word repetition effect in a lexical decision task. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 435–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of certain verbal intrusions in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Dewhurst, S. A. (2001). Category repetition and false recognition: Effects of instance frequency and category size. Journal of Memory & Language, 44, 153–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fillenbaum, S. (1969). Words as feature complexes: False recognition of antonyms and synonyms. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 400–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Freyd, J. J., & Gleaves, D. H. (1996). “Remembering” words not presented in lists: Relevance to the current recovered/false memory controversy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22, 811–813.Google Scholar
  27. Gallo, D. A. (2006). Associative illusions of memory: False memory research in DRM and related tasks. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  28. Gallo, D. A., Roberts, M. J., & Seamon, J. G. (1997). Remembering words not presented in lists: Can we avoid creating false memories? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 271–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gallo, D. A., & Roediger, H. L., III (2002). Variability among word lists in eliciting memory illusions: Evidence for associative activation and monitoring. Journal of Memory & Language, 47, 469–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1–67.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Grossman, L., & Eagle, M. (1970). Synonymity, antonymity, and association in false recognition responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 244–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gunter, R. W., Ivanko, S. L., & Bodner, G. E. (2005). Can test list context manipulations improve recognition accuracy in the DRM paradigm? Memory, 13, 862–873.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Holliday, R. E., & Hayes, B. K. (2000). Dissociating automatic and intentional processes in children’s eyewitness memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 1–42.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Holliday, R. E., & Hayes, B. K. (2001). Automatic and intentional processes in children’s eyewitness suggestibility. Cognitive Development, 16, 617–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Holliday, R. E., & Hayes, B. K. (2002). Automatic and intentional processes in children’s recognition memory: The reversed misinformation effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Holliday, R. E., & Weekes, B. S. (2006). Dissociated developmental trajectories for semantic and phonological false memories. Memory, 14, 624–636.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Hovland, C. I. (1951). Human learning and retention. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 613–689). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  38. Howe, M. L. (2006). Developmentally invariant dissociations in children’s true and false memories: Not all relatedness is created equal. Child Development, 77, 1112–1123.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Howe, M. L. (2007). Children’s emotional false memories. Psychological Science, 18, 856–860.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Howe, M. L. (2008a). Visual distinctiveness and the development of children’s false memories. Child Development, 79, 65–79.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Howe, M. L. (2008b). What is false memory development the development of ? Comment on Brainerd, Reyna, and Ceci (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 134, 768–772.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Hutchison, K. A., & Balota, D. A. (2005). Decoupling semantic and associative information in false memories: Explorations with semantically ambiguous and unambiguous critical lures. Journal of Memory & Language, 52, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Joordens, S., & Besner, D. (1992). Priming effects that span an intervening unrelated word: Implications for models of memory representation and retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 483–491.Google Scholar
  44. Kensinger, E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: The contribution of valence and arousal. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15, 241–251.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Kintsch, W., Welsch, D., Schmalhofer, F., & Zimny, S. (1990). Sentence memory: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Memory & Language, 29, 133–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lampinen, J. M., Copeland, S. M., & Neuschatz, J. S. (2001). Recollections of things schematic: Room schemas revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 1211–1222.Google Scholar
  47. Lampinen, J. M., Leding, J. K., Reed, K. B., & Odegard, T. N. (2006). Global gist extraction in children and adults. Memory, 14, 952–964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Libby, L. K., & Neisser, U. (2001). Structure and strategy in the associative false memory paradigm. Memory, 9, 145–163.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Mandler, G. (1962). From association to structure. Psychological Review, 69, 425–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Masson, M. E. J. (1991). A distributed memory model of context effects in word identification. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 233–263). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  51. McEvoy, C. L., Nelson, D. L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 25, 1177–1194.Google Scholar
  52. McKone, E. (2004). Distinguishing true from false memories via lexical decision as a perceptual implicit test. Australian Journal of Psychology, 56, 42–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1999). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Unpublished manuscript, University of South Florida, Tampa.Google Scholar
  54. Neuschatz, J. S., Benoit, G. E., & Payne, D. G. (2003). Effective warnings in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott false-memory paradigm: The role of identifiability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 29, 35–41.Google Scholar
  55. Odegard, T. N., Holliday, R. E., Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2008). Attention to global gist processing eliminates age effects in false memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 96–113.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  57. Papke, L. E., & Wooldridge, J. (1996). Econometric methods for fractional response variables with an application to 401(k) plan participation rates. Journal of Applied Econometrics, 11, 619–632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Payne, D. G., Elie, C. J., Blackwell, J. M., & Neuschatz, J. S. (1996). Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred. Journal of Memory & Language, 35, 261–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Pezdek, K., Finger, K., & Hodge, D. (1997). Planting false childhood memories: The role of event plausibility. Psychological Science, 8, 437–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Powell, M. B., Roberts, K. P., Ceci, S. J., & Hembrooke, H. (1999). The effects of repeated exposure on children’s suggestibility. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1462–1477.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning & Individual Differences, 7, 1–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Reyna, V. F., & Kiernan, B. (1994). The development of gist versus verbatim memory in sentence recognition: Effects of lexical familiarity, semantic content, encoding instructions, and retention interval. Developmental Psychology, 30, 178–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Reyna, V. F., & Kiernan, B. (1995). Children’s memory and interpretation of psychological metaphors. Metaphor & Symbolic Activity, 10, 309–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Rivers, S. E., Reyna, V. F., & Mills, B. (2008). Risk taking under the influence: A fuzzy-trace theory of emotion in adolescence. Developmental Review, 28, 107–144.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  65. Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented on lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 803–814.Google Scholar
  66. Roediger, H. L., III, Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 385–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Russell, W. A., & Jenkins, J. J. (1954). The complete Minnesota norms for responses to 100 words from the Kent-Rosanoff word association test (Tech. Rep. No. 11, Contract N8 ONR 66216, Office of Naval Research). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
  68. Seamon, J. G., Luo, C. R., Kopecky, J. J., Price, C. A., Rothschild, L., Fung, N. S., & Schwartz, M. A. (2002). Are false memories more difficult to forget than accurate memories? The effect of retention interval on recall and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1054–1064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Seamon, J. G., Luo, C. R., Schwartz, M. A., Jones, K. J., Lee, D. M., & Jones, S. J. (2002). Repetition can have similar or different effects on accurate and false recognition. Journal of Memory & Language, 46, 323–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Sommers, M. S., & Lewis, B. P. (1999). Who really lives next door: Creating false memories with phonological neighbors. Journal of Memory & Language, 40, 83–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Stadler, M. A., Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Norms for word lists that create false memories. Memory & Cognition, 27, 494–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Stahl, C., & Klauer, K. C. (2008). A simplified conjoint recognition paradigm for the measurement of gist and verbatim memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 570–586.Google Scholar
  74. Starns, J. J., & Hicks, J. L. (2005). Source dimensions are retrieved independently in multidimensional monitoring tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31, 1213–1220.Google Scholar
  75. Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory: Mood and the false memory effect. Psychological Science, 16, 785–791.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Thompson-Schill, S. L., Kurtz, K. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Effects of semantic and associative relatedness on automatic priming. Journal of Memory & Language, 38, 440–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Toglia, M. P., & Battig, W. F. (1978). Handbook of semantic word norms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  78. Toglia, M. P., Neuschatz, J. S., & Goodwin, K. A. (1999). Recall accuracy and illusory memories: When more is less. Memory, 7, 233–256.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Tse, C.-S., & Neely, J. H. (2005). Assessing activation without source monitoring in the DRM false memory paradigm. Journal of Memory & Language, 53, 532–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Tussing, A. A., & Greene, R. L. (1999). Differential effects of repetition on true and false recognition. Journal of Memory & Language, 40, 520–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Underwood, B. J. (1965). False recognition produced by implicit verbal responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 122–129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads. Law & Human Behavior, 23, 603–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Wenger, M. J., & Townsend, J. T. (2006). On the costs and benefits of faces and words: Process characteristics of feature search in highly meaningful stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 32, 755–779.Google Scholar
  84. Wu, L. L., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounding concepts in perceptual simulation: I. Evidence from feature generation. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  85. Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory & Language, 46, 441–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Zeelenberg, R., & Pecher, D. (2002). False memories and lexical de cision: Even twelve primes do not cause long-term semantic priming. Acta Psychologica, 109, 269–284.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • C. J. Brainerd
    • 1
  • Y. Yang
    • 1
  • V. F. Reyna
    • 1
  • M. L. Howe
    • 2
  • B. A. Mills
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Human DevelopmentCornell UniversityIthaca
  2. 2.Lancaster UniversityLancasterEngland

Personalised recommendations