Memory & Cognition

, Volume 36, Issue 8, pp 1403–1414 | Cite as

Self-centered memories: The reminiscence bump and the self

  • Clare J. Rathbone
  • Chris J. A. Moulin
  • Martin A. Conway
Article

Abstract

The self-memory relationship is thought to be bidirectional, in such a way that memories provide context for the self, and equally, the self exercises control over retrieval (Conway, 2005). Autobiographical memories are not distributed equally across the life span; instead, memories peak between ages 10 and 30. This reminiscence bump has been suggested to support the emergence of a stable and enduring self. In the present study, the relationship between memory accessibility and self was explored with a novel methodology that used generation of self images in the form of I am statements. Memories generated from I am cues clustered around the time of emergence for that particular self image. We argue that, when a new self-image is formed, it is associated with the encoding of memories that are relevant to that self and that remain highly accessible to the rememberer later in life. This study offers a new methodology for academics and clinicians interested in the relationship between memory and identity.

References

  1. Addis, D. R., & Tippett, L. J. (2004). Memory of myself: Autobiographical memory and identity in Alzheimer’s disease. Memory, 12, 56–74.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barry, E. S., Naus, M. J., & Rehm, L. P. (2006). Depression, implicit memory, and self: A revised memory model of emotion. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 719–745.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2002). Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: The recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and involuntary memories. Psychology & Aging, 17, 636–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Cultural life scripts structure recall from autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 32, 427–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Betz, A. L., & Skowronski, J. J. (1997). Self-events and other-events: Temporal dating and event memory. Memory & Cognition, 25, 701–714.Google Scholar
  6. Bryant, R. A. (1995). Autobiographical memory across personalities in dissociative identity disorder: A case report. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 625–631.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cappeliez, P. (2008). An explanation of the reminiscence bump in the dreams of older adults in terms of life goals and identity. Self & Identity, 7, 25–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen, G., & Faulkner, D. (1988). Life span changes in autobiographical memory. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 1, pp. 277–283). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.Google Scholar
  9. Collins, K. A., Pillemer, D. B., Ivcevic, Z., & Gooze, R. A. (2007). Cultural scripts guide recall of intensely positive life events. Memory & Cognition, 35, 651–659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Conway, M. A. (1996). Autobiographical knowledge and autobiographical memories. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 67–93). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory & Language, 53, 594–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Conway, M. A., & Haque, S. (1999). Overshadowing the reminiscence bump: Memories of a struggle for independence. Journal of Adult Development, 6, 35–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Conway, M. A., & Rubin, D. C. (1993). The structure of autobiographical memory. In A. F. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 103–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Conway, M. A., Singer, J. A., & Tagini, A. (2004). The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence. Social Cognition, 22, 491–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 56, 124–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Elnick, A. B., Margrett, J. A., Fitzgerald, J. M., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (1999). Benchmark memories in adulthood: Central domains and predictors of their frequency. Journal of Adult Development, 6, 45–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  19. Fitzgerald, J. M. (1988). Vivid memories and the reminiscence phenomenon: The role of a self narrative. Human Development, 31, 261–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Glück, J., & Bluck, S. (2007). Looking back across the life span: A life story account of the reminiscence bump. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1928–1939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gordon, C. (1968). Self conceptions: Configurations of content. In C. Gordon & K. J. Gergen (Eds.), The self in social interaction (Vol. 1, pp. 115-136). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  22. Gramzow, R. H., Gaertner, L., & Sedikides, C. (2001). Memory for in-group and out-group information in a minimal group context: The self as an informational base. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80, 188–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grenier, J., Cappeliez, P., St-Onge, M., Vachon, J., Vinette, S., Roussy, F., et al. (2005). Temporal references in dreams and autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 33, 280–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hockey, R. (1973). Rate of presentation in running memory and direct manipulation of input processing strategies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 104–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Holbrook, M. B., & Schindler, R. M. (1989). Some exploratory findings on the development of musical tastes. Journal of Consumer Research, 16, 119–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1988). Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic events: Applications of the schema construct. Social Cognition, 7, 113–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Janssen, S. M. J., Chessa, A. G., & Murre, J. M. J. (2007). Temporal distribution of favourite books, movies, and records: Differential encoding and re-sampling. Memory, 15, 755–767.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Klein, S. B., Cosmides, L., Costabile, K. A., & Mei, L. (2002). Is there something special about the self ? A neuropsychological case study. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 490–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kuhn, M. H., & McPartland, T. S. (1954). An empirical investigation of self-attitudes. American Sociological Review, 19, 68–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Larsen, S. F. (1996). Memorable books: Recall of reading and its personal context. In R. J. Kreuz & M. S. MacNealy (Eds.), Empirical approaches to literature and aesthetics (pp. 583–599). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  31. Lemogne, C., Piolino, P., Friszer, S., Claret, A., Girault, N., Jouvent, R., et al. (2006). Episodic autobiographical memory in depression: Specificity, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective. Consciousness & Cognition, 15, 258–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Loftus, E. F., & Marburger, W. (1983). Since the eruption of Mount St. Helens, has anyone beaten you up? Improving the accuracy of retrospective reports with landmark events. Memory & Cognition, 11, 114–120.Google Scholar
  33. Madson, L., & Trafimow D. (2001). Gender comparisons in the private, collective, and allocentric selves. Journal of Social Psychology, 141, 551–559.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 35, 63–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of the selfconcept. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 51, 858–866.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. McAdams, D. P., Bauer, J. J., Sakaeda, A. R., Anyidoho, N. A., Machado, M. A., Magrino-Failla, K., et al. (2006). Continuity and change in the life story: A longitudinal study of autobiographical memories in emerging adulthood. Journal of Personality, 74, 1371–1400.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McGuire, W. J., McGuire, C. V., Child, P., & Fujioka, T. (1978). Salience of ethnicity in the spontaneous self-concept as a function of one’s ethnic distinctiveness in the social environment. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 36, 511–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. McGuire, W. J., & Padawer-Singer, A. (1976). Trait salience in the spontaneous self-concept. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 33, 743–754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Moffitt, K. T., & Singer, J. A. (1994). Continuity in the life story: Self-defining memories, affect, and approach/avoidance personal strivings. Journal of Personality, 62, 21–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Pillemer, D. B., Goldsmith, L. R., Panter, A. T., & White, S. H. (1988). Very long-term memories of the first year in college. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 14, 709–715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rubin, D. C., & Berntsen, D. (2003). Life scripts help to maintain autobiographical memories of highly positive, but not highly negative, events. Memory & Cognition, 31, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rubin, D. C., Rahaal, T. A., & Poon, L. W. (1998). Things learned in early adulthood are remembered best. Memory & Cognition, 26, 3–19.Google Scholar
  43. Rubin, D. C., Wetzler, S. E., & Nebes, R. D. (1986). Autobiographical memory across the lifespan. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 202–221). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Schrauf, R. W., & Rubin, D. C. (1998). Bilingual autobiographical memory in older adult immigrants: A test of cognitive explanations of the reminiscence bump and the linguistic encoding of memories. Journal of Memory & Language, 39, 437–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Schrauf, R. W., & Rubin, D. C. (2001). Effects of voluntary immigration on the distribution of autobiographical memory over the lifespan. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, S75-S88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Schwartz, S. J. (2006). Predicting identity consolidation from self-construction, eudaimonistic self-discovery, and agentic personality. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 777–793.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sehulster, J. R. (1996). In my era: Evidence for the perception of a special period of the past. Memory, 4, 145–158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Singer, J. A., & Blagov, P. S. (2001). Classification system and scoring manual for self-defining memories. New London, CT: Department of Psychology, Connecticut College.Google Scholar
  49. Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1993). The remembered self: Emotion and memory in personality. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  50. Skowronski, J. J., Ritchie, T. D., Walker, W. R., Betz, A. L., Sedikides, C., Bethencourt, L. A., & Martin, A. L. (2007). Ordering our world: The quest for traces of temporal organization in autobiographical memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 850–856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Waterman, A. S. (1999). Identity, the identity statuses, and identity status development: A contemporary statement. Developmental Review, 19, 591–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Clare J. Rathbone
    • 1
  • Chris J. A. Moulin
    • 1
  • Martin A. Conway
    • 1
  1. 1.Institute of Psychological SciencesUniversity of LeedsLeedsEngland

Personalised recommendations