Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 36, Issue 5, pp 913–919 | Cite as

Can the survival recall advantage be explained by basic memory processes?

  • Yana WeinsteinEmail author
  • Julie M. Bugg
  • Henry L. Roediger
Article

Abstract

Nairne, Thompson, and Pandeirada (2007) demonstrated a striking phenomenon: Words rated for relevance to a grasslands survival scenario were remembered better than identical words encoded under other deep processing conditions. Having replicated this effect using a novel set of words (Experiment 1), we contrasted the schematic processing and evolutionary accounts of the recall advantage (Experiment 2). Inconsistent with the schematic processing account, the grasslands survival scenario produced better recall than did a city survival scenario requiring comparable schematic processing. Recall in the grasslands scenario was unaffected by a self-reference manipulation. The findings are consistent with an evolutionary account that attributes the recall advantage to adaptive memory biases.

Keywords

Inal Interaction Survival Scenario Foreign Land Recall Advantage Part Icipants 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Bower, G. H., & Gilligan, S. G. (1979). Remembering information related to one’s self. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 420–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Challis, B. H., Velichkovsky, B. M., & Craik, F. I. M. (1996). Levelsof-processing effects on a variety of memory tasks: New findings and theoretical implications. Consciousness & Cognition, 5, 142–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. “City” (2007). In Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved May 29, 2007, from Encyclopædia Britannica Online: www.britannica.com/eb/article-9106247.Google Scholar
  5. Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning &Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Fox, E., Griggs, L., & Mouchlianitis, E. (2007). The detection of fear-relevant stimuli: Are guns noticed as quickly as snakes? Emotion, 7, 691–696.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 394–400.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Hyde, T. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1973). Recall for words as a function of semantic, graphic, and syntactic orienting tasks. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 12, 471–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Kang, S. H. K., McDermott, K. B., & Cohen, S. M. (in press). The mnemonic advantage of processing fitness-relevant information. Memory & Cognition.Google Scholar
  11. Kihlstrom, J. F. (1993). What does the self look like? In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Eds.), The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self (pp. 79–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Marks, I. M. (1969). Fears and phobias. London: Heineman.Google Scholar
  13. Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (in press). Adaptive memory: Remembering with a stone-age brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science.Google Scholar
  14. Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Thompson, S. R. (2008). Adaptive memory: The comparative value of survival processing. Psychological Science, 19, 176–180.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2007). Adaptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 263–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Packman, J. L., & Battig, W. F. (1978). Effects of different kinds of semantic processing on memory for words. Memory & Cognition, 6, 502–508.Google Scholar
  18. Roediger, H. L., III, & Gallo, D. A. (2002). Levels of processing: Some unanswered questions. In M. Naveh-Benjamin, M. Moscovitch, & H. L. Roediger III (Eds.), Perspectives on human memory and cognitive aging: Essays in honour of Fergus Craik (pp. 28–47). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  19. Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 35, 677–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2, 307–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–394.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 5–67). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Yana Weinstein
    • 1
    Email author
  • Julie M. Bugg
    • 2
  • Henry L. Roediger
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity College LondonLondonEngland
  2. 2.Washington UniversitySt. Louis

Personalised recommendations