Personality from a controlled processing perspective: An fMRI study of neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness

Article

Abstract

Although neuroticism has been central to most theories of personality, self-reported neuroticism has had limited success in predicting expected behavioral outcomes. The reason for this may be due, in part, to the imprecision of self-reports. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between neural correlates of control systems and neuroticism, extraversion, and self-consciousness. In response to an oddball task, neuroticism was associated with increased dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) reactivity, typically associated with discrepancy detection, whereas extraversion and self-consciousness were associated with lateral and medial frontoparietal networks, respectively, typically associated with task-focused (lateral) or self-focused (medial) controlled processes. We also examined whether the neural measure of neuroticism would predict a relevant behavioral outcome better than self-reports would. Interoceptive accuracy, an outcome associated with neuroticism, was better accounted for by dACC reactivity (r2=.74) than by self-reported neuroticism (r2=.16), suggesting that neural reactivities may provide a more direct measure of personality than self-reports do.

References

  1. Allen, M. T., Stoney, C. M., Owens, J. F., & Matthews, K. A. (1993). Hemodynamic adjustments to laboratory stress: The influence of gender and personality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 505–517.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Ardekani, B. A., Choi, S. J., Hossen-Zadeh, G., Porjesz, B., Tanabe, J. L., Lim, K. O., Bilder, R., Helpern, J. A., & Begleiter, H. (2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of brain activity in the visual oddball task. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 347–356.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Asmundson, G. J. G., Norton, G. R., Wilson, K. G., & Sandler, L. S. (1994). Subjective symptoms and cardiac reactivity to brief hyperventilation in individuals with high anxiety sensitivity. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 32, 237–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bishop, S., Duncan, J., Brett, M., & Lawrence, A. (2004). Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: Controlling attention to threat-related stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 184–188.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., & Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 825–836.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Breiter, H. C., Rauch, S. L., Kwong, K. K., Baker, J. R., Weisskoff, R. M., Kennedy, D. N., Kendrick, A. D., Davis, T. L., Jiang, A., Cohen, M. S., Stern, C. E., Belliveau, J. W., Baer, L., O’Sullivan, R. L., Savage, C. R., Jenike, M. A., & Rosen, B. R. (1996). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 595–606.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bunge, S. A., Hazeltine, E., Scanlon, M. D., Rosen, A. C., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Dissociable contributions of prefrontal and parietal cortices to response selection. NeuroImage, 17, 1562–1571.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bystritsky, A., Pontillo, D., Powers, M., Sabb, F.W., Craske, M. G., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2001). Functional MRI changes during panic anticipation and imagery exposure. NeuroReport, 12, 3953–3957.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Canli, T., Amin, Z., Haas, B., Omura, K., & Constable, R. T. (2004). A double dissociation between mood states and personality traits in the anterior cingulate. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 897–904.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 33–42.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 288, 1835–1838.Google Scholar
  15. Carter, C. S., Macdonald, A. M., Botvinick, M., Ross, L. L., Stenger, V. A., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (2000). Parsing executive processes: Strategic versus evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 1944–1948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control theory approach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  17. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Critchley H. D., Wiens, S., Rothstein, P., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 189–195.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Crosson, B., Sadek, J. R., Maron, L., Gokcay, D., Mor, C. M., Auerback, E. J., Freeman, A. J., Leonard, C. M., & Briggs, R. W. (2001). Relative shift in activity from medial to lateral frontal cortex during internally versus externally guided word generation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 272–283.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Darvill, T. J., Johnson, R. C., & Danko, G. P. (1992). Personality correlates of public and private self consciousness. Personality & Individual Differences, 13, 383–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  23. Ehlers, A., & Breuer, P. (1992). Increased cardiac awareness in panic disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 371–382.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 294–300.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.Google Scholar
  26. Eysenck, S. B. G., & Eysenck, H. J. (1975). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London: Hodder & Stoughton.Google Scholar
  27. Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 43, 522–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 272–273). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  29. Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Gray, J. R., & Braver, T. S. (2002). Personality predicts working-memory-related activation in the caudal anterior cingulate cortex. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 64–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 4259–4264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 685–694.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86–108.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Iacoboni, M., Lieberman, M. D., Knowlton, B. J., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Moritz, M., Throop, C. J., & Fiske, A. P. (2004). Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. NeuroImage, 21, 1167–1173.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Johnson, S. C., Baxter, L. C., Wilder, L. S., Pipe, J. G., Heiserman, J. E., & Prigatano, G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain, 125, 1808–1814.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Kagan J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  38. Kampe, K. K. W., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “mentalizing,” regardless of modality. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5258–5263.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., & Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 785–794.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Kimbrell, T. A., George, M. S., Parekh, P. I., Ketter, T. A., Podell, D. M., Danielson, A. L., Repella, J. D., Benson, B. E., Willis, M. W., Herscovitch, P., & Post, R. M. (1999). Regional brain activity during transient self-induced anxiety and anger in healthy adults. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 454–465.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Kircher, T. T. J., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Rabe-Hesketh, S., Benson, P. J, Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., Bartels, M., & David, A. S. (2001). Recognizing one’s own face. Cognition, 78, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kjaer, K.W., Nowak, M., & Lou, H. C. (2002). Reflective self-awareness and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. NeuroImage, 17, 1080–1086.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Kumari, V., Ffytche, D. H., Williams, S. C., & Gray, J. A. (2004). Personality predicts brain responses to cognitive demands. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 10636–10641.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 819–834.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Lewin, K. (1935). The conflict between Aristotelian and Galileian modes of thought in contemporary psychology. In W. Knows (Ed.). A dynamic theory of personality: Selected papers (pp. 1–42). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  46. Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Introversion and working memory: Central executive differences. Personality & Individual Differences, 28, 479–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lieberman, M. D., & Rosenthal, R. (2001). Why introverts can’t always tell who likes them: Multi-tasking and nonverbal decoding. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80, 294–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Ludwick-Rosenthal, R., & Neufeld, R.W. J. (1985). Heart beat interoception: A study of individual differences. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 57–65.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  50. Matthews, G., & Gilliland, K. (1999). The personality theories of H. J. Eysenck and J. A. Gray: A comparative review. Personality & Individual Differences, 26, 583–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 394–408.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  53. Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638–662.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Roberston, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Gross, J. J. (2004). For better or for worse: Neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage, 23, 483–499.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Osuch, E. A., Ketter, T. A., Kimbrell, T. A., George, M. S., Benson, B. E., Willis, M. W., Herscovitch, P., & Post, R. M. (2000). Regional cerebral metabolism associated with anxiety symptoms in affective disorder patients. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 1020–1023.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Pham, L. B., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2001). Effects of environmental predictability and personal mastery on self-regulatory and physiological processes. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 611–620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 18, 483–488.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Shostak, B. B., & Peterson, R. A. (1990). Effects of anxiety sensitivity on emotional response to a stress task. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 28, 513–521.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283, 1657–1661.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Smith, E. R., & Miller, F. D. (1978). Limits on perception of cognitive processes: A reply to Nisbett and Wilson. Psychological Review, 85, 355–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Stegen, K., Van Diest, I., Van de Woestijne, K. P., & Van den Bergh, O. (2001). Do persons with negative affect have an attentional bias to bodily sensations? Cognition and Emotion, 15, 813–829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Stewart, S. H., Buffett-Jerrott, S. E., & Kokaram, R. (2001). Heartbeat awareness and heart rate reactivity in anxiety sensitivity: A further investigation. Anxiety Disorders, 15, 535–553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Sturges, L. V., & Goetsch, V. L. (1996). Psychophysiological reactivity and heartbeat awareness in anxiety sensitivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 10, 283–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 76, 284–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Ursu, S., Stenger, V. A., Shear, M. K., Jones, M. R., & Carter, C. S. (2003). Overactive action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychological Science, 14, 347–353.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96, 234–254.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Weiner, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Control and communication in the animal and the machine. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  72. Weissman, D. H., Giesbrecht, B., Song, A. W., Mangun, G. R., & Woldorff, M. G. (2003). Conflict monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex during selective attention to global and local object features. NeuroImage, 19, 1361–1368.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CaliforniaLos Angeles

Personalised recommendations