Behavior Research Methods

, Volume 41, Issue 1, pp 213–219 | Cite as

Children’s emotional norms for 600 French words

Article
  • 356 Downloads

Abstract

The present study provides a French child database containing a large corpus of words (N=600) that were rated on emotional valence (positive, neutral, and negative) by French children differing in both age (5, 7, and 9 years old) and sex (girls and boys). Good response reliability was observed in each of the three age groups. The results showed some age differences in the children’s ratings. With increasing age, the percentage of words rated positive decreased, whereas the percentage of neutral words increased and the percentage of negative words remained stable. Our study did not reveal marked differences across sex groups. The database compiled here should become a useful tool for experimental studies in which verbal material is used with children. It would be worthwhile in future research to study how children process emotional words and also to control the emotional variable in the same way as other linguistic variables in the experimental design. The norms from this study may be downloaded from brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Supplementary material

13428_2010_410100213_MOESM1_ESM.zip (130 kb)
Supplementary material, approximately 340 KB.

References

  1. Alario, F.-X., & Ferrand, L. (1999). A set of 400 pictures standardized for French: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 531–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bonin, P., Méot, A., Aubert, L., Malardier, N., Niedenthal, P., & Capelle-Toczek, M.-C. (2003). Normes de concrétude, de valeur d’imagerie, de fréquence subjective et de valence émotionnelle pour 866 mots. L’Année Psychologique, 104, 655–694. doi:10.3406/ psy.2003.29658CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bonin, P., Peereman, R., Malardier, N., Méot, A., & Chalard, M. (2003). A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 158–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. L. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW). Gainesville: University of Florida, NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention.Google Scholar
  5. Cook, G. I., Hicks, J. L., & Marsh, R. L. (2007). Source monitoring is not always enhanced for valenced material. Memory & Cognition, 35, 222–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Daleiden, E. L., & Vasey, M. V. (1997). An information-processing perspective on childhood anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 407–429. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(97)00010-XCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Davidson, D., Luo, Z., & Burden, M. J. (2001). Children’s recall of emotional behaviours, emotional labels, and nonemotional behaviours: Does emotion enhance memory? Cognition & Emotion, 15, 1–26. doi:10.1080/0269993004200105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Denham, S. A., & Couchoud, E. A. (1990). Young preschoolers’ ability to identify emotions in equivocal situations. Child Study Journal, 20, 153–169.Google Scholar
  9. Dewhurst, S. A., & Parry, L. A. (2000). Emotionality, distinctiveness, and recollective experience. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 541–551. doi:10.1080/095414400750050222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Doerksen, S., & Shimamura, A. R. (2001). Source memory enhancement for emotional words. Emotion, 1, 5–11. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.5CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feelings between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 132–139. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ferrand, L., Grainger, J., & New, B. (2003). Normes d’âge d’acquisition pour 400 mots monosyllabiques. L’Année Psychologique, 104, 445–468. doi:10.3406/psy.2003.29645CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Ferré, P. (2003). Effects of level of processing on memory for affectively valenced words. Cognition & Emotion, 17, 859–880. doi:10.1080/02699930302309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Harris, P. L. (1985). What children know about the situations that provoke emotion. In M. Lewis & C. Saarni (Eds.), The socialization of affect (pp. 161–185). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  15. Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory & Cognition, 31, 1169–1180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kissler, J., Assadollahi, R., & Herbert, C. (2006). Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: Insights from ERP studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 147–183.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Lachaud, C. M. (2007). CHACQFAM: Une base de données renseignant l’âge d’acquisition estimé et la familiarité pour 1225 mots monosyllabiques et bisyllabiques du Français. L’Année Psychologique, 1, 39–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lagattuta, K. H., Wellman, H. M., & Flavell, J. H. (1997). Preschoolers’ understanding of the link between thinking and feeling: Cognitive cuing and emotional change. Child Development, 68, 1081–1104.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, M. M. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation and action. In P. J. Lang, R. F. Simons, & M. T. Balaban (Eds.), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97–135). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  20. McManis, M. H., Bradley, M. M., Berg, W. K., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotional reactions in children: Verbal, physiological, and behavioral responses to affective pictures. Psychophysiology, 38, 222–231. doi:10.1017/S0048577201991140CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Messina, D., Morais, J., & Cantraine, F. (1989). Valeur affective de 904 mots de la langue française. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 9, 165–187.Google Scholar
  22. Monnier, C., & Syssau, A. (2008). Semantic contribution to verbal short-term memory: Are pleasant words easier to remember than neutral words in serial recall and serial recognition? Memory & Cognition, 36, 35–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Neshat-Doost, H. T., Moradi, A. R., Taghavi, M. R., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T. (1999). The development of a corpus of emotional words produced by children and adolescents. Personality & Individual Differences, 27, 433–451. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00253-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Neshat-Doost, H. T., Taghavi, M. R., Moradi, A. R., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T. (1998). Memory for emotional trait adjectives in clinically depressed youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 642–650. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.107.4.642CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Niedenthal, P. M., Auxiette, C., Nugier, A., Dalle, N., Bonin, P., & Fayol, M. (2004). A prototype analysis of the French category “émotion.” Cognition & Emotion, 18, 289–312. doi:10.1080/02699930341000086CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  27. Russell, J. A., & Widen, S. C. (2002). A label superiority effect in children’s categorization of facial expressions. Social Development, 11, 30–52. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Stein, N. L., & Levine, L. J. (1989). The causal organisation of emotional knowledge: A developmental study. Cognition & Emotion, 3, 343–378. doi:10.1080/02699938908412712CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Stevenson, R. A., Mikels, J. A., & James, T. W. (2007). Characterization of the Affective Norms for English Words by discrete emotional categories. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 1020–1024.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Syssau, A., & Font, N. (2005). Evaluations des caractéristiques émotionnelles d’un corpus de 604 mots. Bulletin de Psychologie, 58, 361–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Trabasso, T., Stein, N. L., & Johnson, L. R. (1981). Children’s knowledge of events: A causal analysis of story structure. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 15, pp. 237–281). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Vasa, R. A., Carlino, A. R., London, K., & Min, C. (2006). Valence ratings of emotional and non-emotional words in children. Personality & Individual Differences, 41, 1169–1180. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Whissell, C. M., & Nicholson, H. (1991). Children’s freely produced synonyms for seven key emotions. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 72, 1107–1111. doi:10.2466/PMS.72.4.1107-1111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2003). A closer look at preschoolers’ freely produced labels for facial expressions. Developmental Psychology, 39, 114–128. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.114CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2004). The relative power of an emotion’s facial expression, label, and behavioral consequence to evoke preschoolers’ knowledge of its cause. Cognitive Development, 19, 111–125. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Département de PsychologieUniversité Paul ValéryMontpellier Cedex 5France

Personalised recommendations