Advertisement

Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society

, Volume 28, Issue 2, pp 115–118 | Cite as

Healthy older adults’ perceptions of their memory functioning and use of mnemonics

  • Eugene A. Lovelace
  • Paul T. Twohig
Article

Abstract

Interview data were gathered from a purposive sample of 40 healthy, articulate older adults (54 to 85 years old, median = 68 years) concerning the events causing memory problems in their everyday activities, the extent to which they felt they had experienced any change in memory as they grew older, and the sorts of memory aids they employed. The examples of forgetting they provided included both retrospective memory failures (the most common and most irritating being the inability to recall someone’s name) and prospective memory failures (e.g., forgetting to get or do a certain thing while out shopping). For each specific type of memory failure, the modal response was of no change with aging, yet 70% of participants reported an increase in memory failure for one or more items. The memory aids most widely reported involved external mnemonics such as writing notes and placing things in conspicuous places so as not to forget them. The pattern of use of mnemonics parallels other data in the literature for younger adults.

Keywords

Prospective Memory Elderly Adult Memory Event Memory Problem Memory Decline 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Cavanaugh, J. C., Grady, J. G., & Perlmutter, M. (1983). Forgetting and use of memory aids in 20 to 70 year olds everyday life. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 17, 113–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Chaffin, R., & Herrmann, D. J. (1983). Self reports of memory abilities by old and young adults. Human Learning, 2, 17–28.Google Scholar
  3. Crovitz, H. F., & Daniel, W. F. (1984). Measurements of everyday memory: Toward the prevention of forgetting. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22, 413–414.Google Scholar
  4. Dixon, R. A., & Hultsch, D. F. (1983). Structure and development of metamemory in adulthood. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 682–688.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Harris, J. E. (1980). Memory aids people use: Two interview studies. Memory & Cognition, 8, 31–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Hellebrandt, F. A. (1980). Aging among the advantaged: A new look at the stereotype of the elderly. Gerontologist, 20, 404–417.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., & Dixon, R. A. (1987). Age differences in metamemory: Resolving the inconsistencies. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 193–208.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Intons-Peterson, M. J., & Fournier, J. (1986). External and internal memory aids: When and how often do we use them. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 267–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Perlmutter, M. (1978). What is memory aging the aging of? Developmental Psychology, 14, 330–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Sunderland, A., Watts, K., Baddeley, A. D., & Harris, J. E. (1986). Subjective memory assessment and test performance in elderly adults. Journal of Gerontology, 41, 376–384.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Thompson, L. W. (1980). Testing and mnemonic strategies. In L. W. Poon, J. L. Fozard, L. S. Cermak, D. Arenberg, & L. W. Thompson (Eds.), New directions in memory and aging (pp. 367–377). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Williams, S. A., Denney, N. W., & Schadler, M. (1983). Elderly adults’ perception of their own cognitive development during the adult years. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 16, 147–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The psychonomic Society, Inc. 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Eugene A. Lovelace
    • 1
  • Paul T. Twohig
    • 2
  1. 1.Alfred UniversityAlfred
  2. 2.U.S. Army Research InstituteAlexandria

Personalised recommendations