Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 28, Issue 6, pp 1041–1049 | Cite as

Plan formation, retention, and execution in prospective memory: A new approach and age-related effects

  • Matthias Kliegel
  • Mark A. McDanielEmail author
  • Gilles O. Einstein
Article

Abstract

Existing laboratory paradigms of prospective memory instruct subjects to remember to perform a single, isolated act at an appropriate point in the experiment. These paradigms do not completely capture many everyday complex prospective memory situations in which a series or set of delayed actions is planned to be executed in some subsequent period of time. We adapted a laboratory paradigm within which to study these prospective memory processes, and we investigated age-related influences on these prospective memory processes. Age-related declines were found in the planning, initiation, and execution of the set of tasks. In contrast, there were no age differences in plan retention or in the fidelity with which the plan was performed.

Keywords

Prospective Memory Work Memory Capacity Plan Elaboration Prospective Memory Task Retrospective Memory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Bisiacchi, P. S. (1996). The neuropsychological approach in the study of prospective memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.),Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 297–318). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Brandimonte, M., Einstein, G. O., &McDaniel, M. A. (Eds.) (1996).Prospective memory: Theory and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Burgess, P. W. (1996, July).Prospective memory following frontal lobe damage. Paper presented at the Second International Conference on Memory, Abano-Padua, Italy.Google Scholar
  4. Burgess, P. W., &Shallice, T. (1997). The relationship between prospective and retrospective memory: Neuropsychological evidence. In M. A. Conway (Ed.),Cognitive models of memory (pp. 247–272). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. Byrne, R. (1977). Planning meals: Problem-solving on a real database.Cognition,5, 287–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Charness, N. (1981). Search in chess: Age and skill differences.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,7, 467–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cherry, K. E., &LeCompte, D. C. (1999). Age and individual differences in prospective memory.Psychology & Aging,14, 60–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cohen, R. L. (1981). On the generality of some memory laws.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,85, 273–282.Google Scholar
  9. Cohn, N. B., Dustman, R. E., &Bradford, D. C. (1984). Age-related decrements in Stroop color test performance.Journal of Clinical Psychology,40, 1244–1250.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Craik, F. I. M., &Jennings, J. M. (1992). Human memory. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.),The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 51–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  11. Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging.Developmental Review,12, 45–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dobbs, A. R., &Reeves, M. B. (1996). Prospective memory: More than memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.),Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 199–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  13. Dougherty, M. R. P., Gronlund, S. D., Canning, J. M., Durso, F. T., & Mills, S. H. (1998, May).Plan generation in air-traffic control. Paper presented at the Tenth International Symposium on Aviation Psychology, Columbus, OH.Google Scholar
  14. Einstein, G. O., Holland, L. J., McDaniel, M. A., &Guynn, M. J. (1992). Age-related deficits in prospective memory: The influence of task complexity.Psychology & Aging,7, 471–478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Einstein, G. O., &McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 717–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Richardson, S. L., Guynn, M. J., &Cunfer, A. R. (1995). Aging and prospective memory: Examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 996–1007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Einstein, G. O., Smith, R. E., McDaniel, M. A., &Shaw, P. (1997). Aging and prospective memory: The influence of increased task demands at encoding and retrieval.Psychology & Aging,12, 479–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ellis, J. A. (1996). Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.),Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 1–22). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Ellis, J. A., & Shallice, T. (1993).Memory for, and the organization of, future intentions. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  20. Engelkamp, J. (1991). Memory of action events: Some implications for memory theory and for imagery. In C. Cornoldi & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.),Imagery & cognition (pp. 183–219). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  21. Guynn, M. J., McDaniel, M. A., &Einstein, G. O. (1998). Prospective memory: When reminders fail.Memory & Cognition,26, 287–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Guynn, M. J., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (in press). Remembering to perform actions: A different type of memory? In H. D. Zimmer & R. L. Cohen (Eds.),Memory for action: A distinct form of episodic memory? New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., &Rypma, B. (1991). Age and inhibition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 163–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hasher, L., &Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  25. Houx, P. J., Jolles, J., &Vreeling, F. W. (1993). Stroop interference: Aging effects assessed with the Stroop color—word test.Experimental Aging Research,19, 209–224.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Kvavilashvili, L., &Chitashvili, M. (1991). Forgetting intentions as a stable personality characteristic and the problem of its quantitative measurement.Matsne: Series in Philosophy & Psychology,3, 67–78 [in Georgian].Google Scholar
  27. Lachman, M. E., &Burack, O. R. (1993). Planning and control processes across the life span: An overview.International Journal of Behavioral Development,16, 131–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lehrl, S. (1977).Der Mehrfachwahlwortschatztest MWT-B. Erlangen: Straube.Google Scholar
  29. Light, L. L. (1991). Memory and aging: Four hypotheses in search of data.Annual Review of Psychology,42, 333–376.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Marsh, R. L., &Hicks, J. L. (1998). Event-based prospective memory and executive control of working memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,24, 336–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., &Landau, J. D. (1998). An investigation of everyday prospective memory.Memory & Cognition,26, 633–643.Google Scholar
  32. Martin, M., &Ewert, O. (1997). Attention and planning in older adults.International Journal of Behavioral Development,20, 577–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Martin, M., &Schumann-Hengsteler, R. (1996). Prospective memory in old age: An overview, practical suggestions, and future directions.Eichstätter Berichte zur Entwicklungs- und Paedagogischen Psychologie (No. 6). Eichstaett, Germany: University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Maylor, E. A. (1996). Age-related impairment in an event-based prospective memory task.Psychology & Aging,11, 74–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. McDaniel, M. A. (1995). Prospective memory: Progress and processes. In D. Medin (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 33, pp. 191–221). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  36. McDaniel, M. A., &Einstein, G. O. (1993). The importance of cue familiarity and cue distinctiveness in prospective memory.Memory,1, 23–41.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., Manzi, M., & Cochran, B. (1998).Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory: A multiprocess framework. Unpublished manuscript, University of New Mexico, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
  38. McDaniel, M. A., Robinson-Riegler, B., &Einstein, G. O. (1998). Prospective remembering: Perceptually driven or conceptually driven processes?Memory & Cognition,26, 121–134.Google Scholar
  39. Park, D. C., Hertzog, C., Kidder, D. P., Morell, R.W., &Mayhorn, C. B. (1997). Effect of age on event-based and time-based prospective memory.Psychology & Aging,12, 314–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing.Psychological Science,2, 179–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Aging associations: Influence of speed on adult age differences in associative learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,20, 1486–1503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Salthouse, T. A., &Meinz, E. J. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed.Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,50B, P297-P306.Google Scholar
  43. Shallice, T., &Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man.Brain,114, 727–741.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Smith, A. D., &Earles, J. L. K. (1996). Memory changes in normal aging. In F. Blanchard-Fields & T. M. Hess (Eds.),Perspectives on cognitive change in adulthood and aging (pp. 192- 220). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  45. Somerville, S. C., Wellmann, H. M., &Cultice, J. C. (1983). Young children’s deliberate reminding.Journal of Genetic Psychology,143, 87–96.Google Scholar
  46. Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., &Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,22, 461–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.Journal of Experimental Psychology,18, 643–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Waters, G. S., &Caplan, D. (1996). The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,49A, 51–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Wechsler, D. (1981).WAIS—R manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  50. West, R. L. (1988). Prospective memory and aging. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.),Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues. Vol. 2: Clinical and educational implications (pp. 119–125). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.Google Scholar
  51. Wilson, B. A., Cockburn, J., &Baddeley, A. D. (1985).The Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test. Titchfield, U.K.: Thames Valley Test Co.Google Scholar
  52. Zacks, R. T., &Hasher, L. (1994). Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.),Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 241–264). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  • Matthias Kliegel
    • 1
  • Mark A. McDaniel
    • 2
    Email author
  • Gilles O. Einstein
    • 3
  1. 1.University of HeidelbergHeidelbergGermany
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of New MexicoAlbuquerque
  3. 3.Furman UniversityGreenville

Personalised recommendations