Perception & Psychophysics

, Volume 57, Issue 7, pp 962–970 | Cite as

Perception of relative pitch with different references: Some absolute-pitch listeners can’t tell musical interval names

Article

Abstract

Two experiments were conducted to examine the effect of absolute-pitch possession on relativepitch processing. Listeners attempted to identify melodic intervals ranging from a semitone to an octave with different reference tones. Listeners with absolute pitch showed declined performance when the reference was out-of-tune C, out-of-tune E, or F#, relative to when the reference was C. In contrast, listeners who had no absolute pitch maintained relatively high performance in all reference conditions. These results suggest that absolute-pitch listeners are weak in relative-pitch processing and show a tendency to rely on absolute pitch in relative-pitch tasks.

References

  1. Attneave, F., &Olson, R. K. (1971). Pitch as medium: A new approach to psychophysical scaling.American Journal of Psychology,84, 147–166.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bachem, A. (1937). Various types of absolute pitch.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,9, 146–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Benguerel, A. P., &Westdal, C. (1991). Absolute pitch and perception of sequential musical intervals.Music Perception,9, 105–120.Google Scholar
  4. Brady, P. T. (1970). Fixed-scale mechanism of absolute pitch.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,48, 883–887.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Burns, E. M., &Ward, W. D. (1978). Categorical perception-Phenomenon or epiphenomenon: Evidence from experiments in the perception of melodic musical intervals.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,63, 456–468.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Corliss, E. L. (1973). Remark on “Fixed-scale mechanism of absolute pitch”.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,53, 1737–1739.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Cuddy, L. L. (1968). Practice effects in the absolute judgment of pitch.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,43, 1069–1076.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Hulse, S. H. (1989). Comparative psychology and pitch pattern perception in songbirds. In R. J. Dooling & S. H. Hulse (Eds.),The comparative psychology of audition: Perceiving complex sounds (pp. 331–349). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Hulse, S. H., Takeuchi, A. H., &Braaten, R. F. (1992). Perceptual invariances in the comparative psychology of music.Music Perception,10, 151–184.Google Scholar
  10. Krumhansl, C. L. (1990).Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Krumhansl, C. L., &Kessler, E. J. (1982). Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys.Psychological Review,89, 334–368.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Krumhansl, C. L., &Shepard.R. N. (1979). Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,5, 579–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lerdahl, F. (1988). Tonal pitch space.Music Perception,5, 315–350.Google Scholar
  14. Miyazaki, K. (1988). The identification of musical pitch by absolute pitch possessors.Perception & Psychophysics,44, 501–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Miyazaki, K. (1989). Absolute pitch identification: Effects of timbre and pitch region.Music Perception,7, 1–14.Google Scholar
  16. Miyazaki, K. (1990). The speed of musical pitch identification by absolute-pitch possessors.Music Perception,8, 177–188.Google Scholar
  17. Miyazaki, K. (1992). Perception of musical intervals by absolute pitch possessors.Music Perception,9, 413–426.Google Scholar
  18. Oura, Y., &Eguchi, K. (1982). Absolute pitch training program for children.Ongaku Kyouiku Kenkyu (Music Education Research),32, 162–171. (in Japanese)Google Scholar
  19. Piston, W. (1987).Harmony (5th ed.). New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  20. Rakowski, A. (1990). Intonation variants of musical intervals in isolation and in musical contexts.Psychology of Music,18, 60–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Sergeant, D., &Roche, S. (1973). Perceptual shifts in the auditory information processing of young children.Psychology of Music,1, 39–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Siegel, J. A., &Siegel, W. (1977). Absolute identification of notes and intervals by musicians.Perception & Psychophysics,21, 143–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Takeuchi, A. H., &Hulse, S. H. (1991). Absolute-pitch judgments of black- and white-key pitches.Music Perception,9, 27–46.Google Scholar
  24. Takeuchi, A. H., &Hulse, S. H. (1993). Absolute pitch.Psychological Bulletin,113, 345–361.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Ward, W. D., &Burns, E. M. (1982). Absolute pitch. In D. Deutsch (Ed.),The psychology of music (pp. 431–451). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Faculty of HumanitiesNiigata UniversityNiigataJapan

Personalised recommendations