Memory & Cognition

, Volume 22, Issue 1, pp 95–110 | Cite as

Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning

Article

Abstract

In three experiments, we studied the relation between degree of implicit learning and two aspects of short-term memory: (l) the activation level of the to-be-learned information, and (2) individual differences in short-term memory capacity. In all the experiments, we used the Nissen and Bullemer (1987) serial reaction time paradigm or a modification thereof. The effects of activation level were assessed by experimentally manipulating the rate of presentation. Individual differences in short-term memory capacity were assessed via traditional span measures. The experiments demonstrated that the rate of presentation reliably affected an indirect measure of learning (i.e., response time) under both incidental and intentional task instructions and under both single-task and dual-task conditions. Short-term memory span was reliably related to the indirect measure of learning only in some experimental conditions. The findings represent important constraints for models of implicit serial learning and are discussed within a general framework for understanding implicit learning and memory.

References

  1. Arenberg, D (1982). Learning from our nustakes in aging research.Experimental Aging Research,8, 73–76.Google Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A D. (1986).Working memory Oxford Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A D. (1992). Working memoryScience,255, 556–559CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Berry, D C., &Broadbent, D E. (1984) On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalizable knowledge.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,36A, 209–231.Google Scholar
  5. Berry, D. C, &Broadbent, D E. (1988) Interactive tasks and the implicit-explicit distinction.British Journal of Psychology,79, 251–272Google Scholar
  6. Berry, D. C., &Dienes, Z. (1991) The relationship between implicit memory and implicit learningBritish Journal of Psychology,82, 359–373Google Scholar
  7. Botwinick, J., &Storandt, M. (1974)Memory, related-functions and age. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
  8. Broadbent, D. E. (1984) The Maltese cross A new simplistic model for memory.Behavioural & Brain Sciences,7, 55–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chiarello, C., &Hover, W. J. (1988). Adult age differences in unpheit and explicit memory: Time course and encoding effects.Psychology & Aging,3, 358–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cleeremans, A., &McClelland, J. L (1991) Learning the structure of event sequences.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,120, 235–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cohen, A Ivry, R. I., &Keelf, S W (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 17–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing systemPsychological Bulletin,104, 163–191CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Cowan, N (1993). Activation, attention, and short-term memory.Memory & Cognition,21, 152–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Curran, T, &KeeLe, S W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning.Journal of Experimenral Psychology Learning, Memory, & Cognition,19, 189–202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dienes, Z. (1992). Cannectionist and memory-array models of artificialgraftunar learning Cognitive Science,16, 41–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Estes, W K. (1976). The cognitive side of probability learning,Psychological Review,83, 37–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Estes, W. K. (1986). Memory storage and retrieval processes in category learning.Journal of Eapenmenml Psychology: General,115, 155–174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Falmagne, J C. (1965) Stochastic models for choice reaction time with application to experimental results.Journal of Mathemancal Psychology,2, 77–124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Fendrich, D.M., Healy, A. F, &Bourne, L E. (1991). Long-term repetition effects for motoric and perceptual proceduresJournal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 137–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Frensch, P A (1991). Transfer of composed knowledge in a multistep serial task.Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 997–1016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Frensch, P. A. (in press). Composition dating renal learning A serial position effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, & CognitionGoogle Scholar
  22. Frensch, P A, Buchner, A, & Lin, J (in press) Implicit learning of unique and ambiguous serial transitions in the presence and absence of a distractor task.Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Meman, & Cognition.Google Scholar
  23. Frensch, P A., &Gerry, D. C. (1993). The effects of practice on component processes in complex mental addition.Journal of Expenmental Psychology Learning, Memory, & Cognition,19, 433–456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Goschke, T (1992, April)The role of attention in implicit learning of structured event sequences. Paper presented at the Annual Conference for Experimental Psychologists, Osnabruck, Germany.Google Scholar
  25. Hartman, M, Knopman, D S., &Nissen, M J. (1989) Implicit learning of new verbal associations.Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, & Cognition,15, 1070–1082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hayves, N A., &Broadbent, D. E (1988). Two modes of learning for interactive tasksCognition,28, 249–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hebb, D O. (1944)The organization of behavior A neuropsycholagical theory. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  28. Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In A. Fessard, R W Gerard, J. Konorsky, & J F. Delafresnaye (Eds),Brain mechanisms and learning (pp. 37–51). Oxford BlackwellGoogle Scholar
  29. Hinszman, D L (1990). Human learning and memory: Connections and dissociations.Annual Review of Psychology,41, 109–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Howard, D. V, &Howard, J. H, Jr (1989). Age differences in learning serial patterns: Direct versus indirect measures.Psychology & Aging,4, 357–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Howard, D. V, &Howard, J H., Jr (1992). Adult age differences in the rate of learning serial patterns: Evidence from direct and indirect tests.Psychology & Aging,7,232–2411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Howard, L. H, Jr.,Mutter, S. A., &Howard, D V. (1992). Serial pattern learning by event observation.Journal of Experimenml Psychology. Learning, Memory, & Cognition,18,1029–10399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jacoby, L L, &Brooks, L. R. (1984). Nonanalytic cognition Memory, perception, and concept learning. In G. Bower (Ed),The psychology of learning and motivation Vol. 18 (pp. 1–47). New York; Academic Press.Google Scholar
  34. Jacoby, L. L, Toth, J P, &Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring recollection.Journal of Experimental Psychology General,122, 139–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jordan, M. L (1986). Attractor dynamics and parallelism in a connectionist sequential machine. InEighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 531–546). Hillsdale, NJ. Erlbanm.Google Scholar
  36. Jordan, M. L, &Rumelhart, D. E (1992) Forward models Supervised learning with a distal teacher.Cognitive Science,16, 307–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kausler, D. H (1991).Experimental psycholagy, cognition, and human aging New York. Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  38. Keele, S W., &Jennings, P.J. (1992). Attention in the representation of sequence Experiment and theoryHuman Movement Science,11, 125–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Keele, S W., &Summers, I G. (1976). Structure of motor programs In G. E Stelmach (Ed ),Motor control: Issues and trends (pp. 109–142) New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
  40. Knopman, D. S. (1991) Unaware learning versus preserved learning in pharmacological amnesia Similarities and differences.Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 1017–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Knopman, D. S., &Nissen, M J. (1987) Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease.Neurology,37, 784–788PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Laming, D. R. J. (1969). Subjective probability in choice-reaction experimentsJournal of Mathematical Psychology,6, 81–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lewicki, P., Czyzewska, M, &Hoffman, H. (1987). Unconscious acquisition of complex procedural knowledge.Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, & Cognition,13, 523–530CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Lewicki, P, Hill, T, &Hizot, E (1488). Acquisition of procedural knowledge about apattern of stimuli that cannot he articulated.Cognitive Psychology,20, 24–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Light, L L., &Anderson, P A (1985) Working-memory capacity, age, and memory for discourse.Journal of Gerontology,411, 737–747.Google Scholar
  46. Light.L L, &Burke, D M (1988). Patterns of language and memory in old age in L Light & D Burke (Eds.),Language, memory, and aging (pp. 244–271) Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  47. Lin, J, Frensch, P. A, & Suchner, A (1993)Implicit serial learning in the presence and absence o fa distractor task Manuscript in preparationGoogle Scholar
  48. Mathews, R C., Buss, R R, Stanley, W B., Blanchard-Fields, F., Cho, J. R, &Druhan, B (1989). Role of implicit and explicit processes to learning from examples. A synergistic effectJournal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, & Cognition,15, 1083–1100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. McKeolvie, S. J. (1987). Learning and awareness in the Hebb digits task.Journal of General Psychology,114, 75–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Miliward, R. B, &Reber A S. (1968) Event recall in probability learning.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,7, 980–984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Miliward, R. B, &Reber A S. (1972) Probability learning: contingent-event schedules with Lags AmericanJournal of Psychology,85, 81–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Myers, J. L (1979)Fundamentals of experimental design Boston Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  53. Nelson, D L, Schreider, T A., &McEvoy, C. L. (1942) Processing implicit and explicit representationsPsychological Review,99, 322–348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Nissen, M J., &Bullemer, P. (1987) Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measuresCognitive Psychology,19, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Nissen, M. I., Knopman, D S., &Schacter, D L (1987) Neurochemical dissociation of memory systemsNeurology,3, 789–793.Google Scholar
  56. Nissen, M, I, Willingham, D. B., &Hartman, M. (1989). Explicit and implicit remembering: When is learning preserved to amnesia?Nearopsychologia,27, 341–352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Peruchet, P., &Amorim, M -A (1992) Conscious knowledge and changes in performance in sequence learning: Evidence against dissociation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,18, 785–800CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Plude, D. J, &Hoyer, W J (1985) Attention and performance Identifying and localizing age deficits. In N Charriess (Ed),Aging and human perfornrance (pp. 47–100) New York WileyGoogle Scholar
  59. Reber, A. S. (1467) Implicit learning of artificial grammarsJournal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,6,855–8633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Reber, A S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,118, 219–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Remington, R. J. (1969) Analysis of sequential effects in choice reaction timesJournal of Experimental Psychology,82, 250–257.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Richardson-Klavehn, A, &Bjork, R. A (1988) Measures of memoryAnnual Review of Psychology,39, 475–543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Roediger, H L., III (1990) Implicit memory Retention without awarenessAmerican Psychologist,45, 1043–1056CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Rose, T. L, Yesayage, J. A, Hill, R. D, &Bower, G. H (1986). Priming effects and recognition memory in young and elderly adults.Experimental Aging Research,12, 31–37PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Salthouse, T A. (1985)A theory of cognitive aging Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
  66. Salthouse, T A. (1991). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing.Psychological Science,2, 174–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Schacter, D L. (1987) Implicit memory History and current status.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,13, 501–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Servan-Schreider, E., &Anderson, J. R. (1990) Learning artificial grammars with competitive chunking.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 592–608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Stadler, M. A (1989) On learning complex procedural knowledge.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,15, 1061–1069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Stadler, M A. (1942) Statistical structure and implicit send learningJournal of Experimerual Psychology. Learning, Memory, & Cognition.18, 318–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Willingham, D. H., Nissen, M J, &Bullemer, P. (1489) On the development of procedural knowledge.Journal of Experimental Psycholaqy. Learning, Memory, & Cognition,15, 1047–1060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Winer, B. J. (1971)Statistical principles to experimental design (2nd rd) New York McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  73. Wright, B M, &Payne, R B (1985). Effects of aging on sex differences. in psychomotor reminiscence and tracking proficiencyJournal of Gerontology,40, 179–184.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1994

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MissounColumbia

Personalised recommendations