Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 20, Issue 6, pp 715–726 | Cite as

The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon: Blocking or partial activation?

  • Antje S. Meyer
  • Kathryn Bock
Article
  • 1.8k Downloads

Abstract

Tip-of-the-tongue states may represent the momentary unavailability of an otherwise accessible word or the weak activation of an otherwise inaccessible word. In three experiments designed to address these alternative views, subjects attempted to retrieve rare target words from their definitions. The definitions were followed by cues that were related to th.e targets in sound, by cues that were related in meaning, and by cues that were not related to the targets. Experiment 1 found that compared with unrelated cues, related cue words that were presented immediately after target definitions helped rather than hindered lexical retrieval, and that sound cues were more effective retrieval aids than meaning cues. Experiment 2 replicated these results when cues were presented after an initial target-retrieval attempt. These findings reverse a previous one (Jones, 1989) that was reproduced in Experiment 3 and shown to stem from a small group of unusually difficult target definitions.

Keywords

Target Word Correct Target Unrelated Condition Response Sheet Lexical Retrieval 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Bock, J. K. (1987). Coordinating words and syntax in speech plans. In A. Ellis (Ed.),Progress in the psychology of language (pp. 337–390). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Brennen, T., Baguley, T., Bright, J., &Bruce, V. (1990). Resolving semantically induced tip-of-the-tongue states for proper nouns.Memory & Cognition,18, 339–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brown, A. S. (1991). A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience.Psychological Bulletin,109, 204–223.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brown, R., &Mcneill, D. (1966). The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon,Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,5, 325–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Burke, D. M., Mackay, D. G., Worthley, J. S., &Wade, E. (1991). On the tip of the tongue: What causes word finding failures in young and older adults?Journal of Memory & Language,30, 542–579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burnage, A. G. (1990).Celex: A guide for users. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: University of Nijmegen.Google Scholar
  7. Collins, A. M., &Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing.Psychological Review,82, 407–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dell, G. S. (1986), A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production.Psychological Review,93, 283–321.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Freedman, J. L., &Landauer, T. K. (1966). Retrieval of long-term memory: “Tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon.Psychonomic Science,4, 309–310.Google Scholar
  10. Garrett, M. F. (1988). Processes in language production, in F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.),Linguistics: The Cambridge survey: III. Language: Psychological and biological aspects (pp. 69–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Grainger, J. (1990). Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming.Journal of Memory & Language,29, 228–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gruneberg, M. M., &Monks, J. (1974). “Feeling of knowing” and cued recall.Acta Psycholagica,38, 257–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Jones, G. V. (1989). Back to Woodworm.: Role of interlopers in the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon.Memory & Cognition,17, 69–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Jones, G. V., &Langford, S. (1987). Phonological blocking in the tip of the tongue state.Cognition,26, 115–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jonides, J,, & Mack, R. (1984). On the cost and benefit of cost and benefit.Psychological Bulletin,96, 29–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kirk, R. E. (1968).Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  17. Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Effects of distorted auditory and of rhyming cues on retrieval of tip-of-the-tongue words by poets and non-poets.Memory & Cognition,5, 477–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Levelt, W. J. M. (1989).Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  19. Mackay, D. G. (1987).The organization of perception and action. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  20. Maylor, E. A. (1990). Age, blocking and the tip of the tongue state.British Journal of Psychology,81, 123–134.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Pease, D. M., &Goodglass, H. (1978). The effects of cuing on picture naming in aphasia.Cortex,14, 178–189.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Ratcliff, R-, & Mckoon, G. (1988). A retrieval theory of priming in memory.Psychological Review,95, 385–408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Reason, J., &Lucas, D. (1983), Using cognitive diaries to investigate naturally occurring memory blocks. In J. E. Harris & P. E. Morris (Eds.),Everyday memory, actions and absent-mindedness (pp. 53–70). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  24. Roget's International Thesaurus (4th ed.) (1977). London: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  25. Roget's Thesaurus of English Words and Phrases. (1982). Harlow, U.K.: Longman Group.Google Scholar
  26. Rubin, D. C, &Wallace, W. T, (1989). Rhyme and reason: Analyses of dual retrieval cues.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, A Cognition,15, 698–709,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Stemberger, J. P. (1985). An interactive activation model of language production, In A. Ellis (Ed.),Progress in the psychology of language (pp. 143–186). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  28. Webster's Collegiate Thesaurus. (1976). Springfield, MA: G&C Merriam.Google Scholar
  29. Webster's New Collegiate Dictionary. (1981). Springfield, MA: G&C Merriam.Google Scholar
  30. Woodworth, R. S. (1929).Experimental psychology. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  31. Woodworth, R. S. (1938).Experimental psychology. New York: Holt,Google Scholar
  32. Yantv, I., &Meyer, D. E. (1987). Activation and metacognition of inaccessible stored information: Potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,13, 187–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Antje S. Meyer
    • 1
  • Kathryn Bock
    • 2
  1. 1.Max Planck Institute for PsycholinguistiesNijmegenThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of IllinoisChampaign

Personalised recommendations