Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 17, Issue 6, pp 693–700 | Cite as

Secondary memory and very rapid forgetting

  • Marc M. Sebrechts
  • Richard L. Marsh
  • John G. Seamon
Article

Abstract

Studies of recall in the absence of expectancy (e. g., Muter, 1980) have suggested that forgetting from primary memory is much more rapid than previously assumed. Two experiments examined the role of secondary memory, as reflected by encoding strategies, in determining this rate of forgetting. Experiment 1 demonstrated that the type of encoding specified by orienting tasks can influence recall in a traditional Brown-Peterson task. Experiment 2 demonstrated a similar pattern of effects of orienting task in the Muter task when recall was not expected, despite much more rapid forgetting. The type of encoding engaged by the orienting tasks did not account for Muter-s results. Expectancy and orienting task appear to have separable influences on resource allocation during encoding. The presence of secondary memory influences at even the shortest retention interval indicates that forgetting from primary memory may be even more rapid than has been proposed.

Keywords

Retention Interval Critical Trial Semantic Group Primary Memory Secondary Memory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Barnard, P. (1985). Interacting cognitive subsystems: A psycholinguistic approach to short-term memory. In A. Ellis (Ed. ),Progress in the psychology of language (Vol. 2, pp. 197–258). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Brown, J. (1958). Some test of the decay theory of immediate memory.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,10, 12–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cermak, L. S., &Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.) (1979).Levels of processing in human memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Craik, F., &Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Crowder, R. G. (1976).Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Daniels, A. H. (1895). Memory, after-image, and attention.American Journal of Psychology,6, 558–564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Healy, A. F., Fendrich, D. W., Cunningham, T. F., &Till, R. E. (1987). Effects of cuing on short-term retention of order information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,13, 413–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Jacoby, L. L., &Bartz, W. H. (1972). Encoding processes and the negative recency effect.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 561–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Keppel, G., &Underwood, B. (1962). Proactive inhibition in shortterm retention of single items.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,1, 153–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Loftus, G. R. (1985). Evaluating forgetting curves.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,11, 397–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Mazuryk, G. F. (1974). Positive recency in final free recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology,103, 812–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Mazuryk, G. F., &Lockhart, R. S. (1974). Negative recency and levels of processing in free recall.Canadian Journal of Psychology,28, 114–123.Google Scholar
  13. Murdock, B. B. (1961). The retention of individual items.Journal of Experimental Psychology,62, 618–625.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Muter, P. (1980). Very rapid forgetting.Memory & Cognition,8, 174–179.Google Scholar
  15. Peterson, L. R., &Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items.Journal of Experimental Psychology,58, 193–198.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Schneider, W., &Detweiler, M. (1987). A connectionist/control architecture for working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 21, pp. 53–119). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  17. Seamon, J. G., Schooler, L. J., Sklar, E., & Sebrechts, M. M. (1987, April).The role of proactive interference in very rapid forgetting. Paper presented the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Arlington, VA.Google Scholar
  18. Shulman, H. G. (1972). Semantic confusion errorsin short-term memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Behavior,11, 221–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Smith, E. E., Barresi, J., &Gross, A. E. (1971). Imaginai versus verbal coding and the primary-secondary memory distinction.Journal of Verbal Learning & Behavior,10, 597–603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Thorndike, E. L., &Lorge, I. (1944).The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Watkins, M. J., &Watkins, O. C. (1974). A tactile suffix effect.Memory & Cognition,2, 176–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Waugh, N. C., &Norman, D. A. (1965). Primary memory.Psychological Review,12, 89–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Wickens, D. D., Born, D. G., &Allen, C. K. (1963). Proactive inhibition and item similarity in short-term memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,2, 440–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marc M. Sebrechts
    • 1
    • 2
  • Richard L. Marsh
    • 1
  • John G. Seamon
    • 1
  1. 1.Wesleyan UniversityMiddletown
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyThe Catholic University of AmericaWashington, DC

Personalised recommendations