Advertisement

Perception & Psychophysics

, Volume 38, Issue 2, pp 115–124 | Cite as

Time perception and attention: The effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration

  • Scott W. Brown
Article

Abstract

This research was designed to compare time judgments obtained under prospective conditions (in which subjects are instructed to attend to time) and retrospective conditions (in which subjects are unaware that they will be required to judge time). In Experiment 1, subjects prospectively or retrospectively judged the duration of intervals spent performing a perceptual-motor task at different levels of difficulty. The results showed that subjects tested under both research paradigms tended to give increasingly shorter and/or more inaccurate time judgments with increases in nontemporal task demands. Experiment 2 was designed to test the effects of attentional deployment on perceived time by comparing prospective and retrospective judgments under control, selective attention, and divided attention conditions. Both types of time judgments became increasingly inaccurate as attention was more broadly deployed. The results of these experiments are consistent with an attentional allocation model, and they suggest that nontemporal task demands disrupt or interfere with timing in both prospective and retrospective situations.

Keywords

Word Pair Task Condition Verbal Estimation Ratio Score Time Judgment 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Allen, D. A. (1980). Filling time versus affective response to the activity that fills the time: Independent effects on time judgment?Perceptual and Motor Skills,51, 723–727.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Axel, R. (1924). Estimation of time.Archives of Psychology,12, 1–72.Google Scholar
  3. Bakan, P. (1955). Effect of set and work speed on time estimation.Perceptual & Motor Skills,5, 147–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Block, R. A. (1979). Time and consciousness. In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.),Aspects of consciousness (Vol. 1 ), pp 179–217. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  5. Block, R. A., George, E. J., &Reed, M. A (1980) A watched pot sometimes boils: A study of duration experience.Acta Psychologica,46, 81–94.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Block, R. A., &Reed, M. A. (1978). Remembered duration: Evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,4, 656–665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brown, S.W. (1984, May).Cognitive effort and temporal information processing. Paper presented at the scientific meeting of the Maine Psychological Association, Bates College, Lewiston, ME.Google Scholar
  8. Burnside, W. (1971). Judgment of short time intervals while performing mathematical tasks,Perception & Psychophysics,9, 404–406.Google Scholar
  9. Cahoon, D., &Edmonds, E. M. (1980). The watched pot still won’t boil: Expectancy as a variable in estimating the passage of time.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,16, 115–116.Google Scholar
  10. Carlson, V. R., &Feinberg, I. (1970). Time judgment as a function of method, practice, and sex.Journal of Experimental Psychology,85, 171–180.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Clausen, J. (1950). An evaluation of experimental methods of time judgment.Journal of Experimental Psychology,40, 756–762.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cohen, J. (1966). Subjective time. In J. T. Fraser (Ed.),The voices of time (pp. 257–275). New York: Braziller.Google Scholar
  13. Cohen, S. (1971). Effects of task, interval and order of presentation on time estimations.Perceptual and Motor Skills,33, 101–102.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Curton, E. D., &Lordahl, D. S. (1974). Effects of attentional focus and arousal on time estimation.Journal of Experimental Psychology,103, 861–867.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Dewolfe, R. K. S., &Duncan, C. P. (1959) Time estimation as a function of level of behavior of successive tasks.Journal of Experimental Psychology,58, 153–158CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Doob, L. W. (1971).Patterning of time. New Haven Yale University PressGoogle Scholar
  17. Fowler, C. A (1979). “Perceptual centers” in speech production and perception.Perception & Psychophysics,25, 375–388.Google Scholar
  18. ]Fraisse, P (1963).The psychology of time. New York: Harper & RowGoogle Scholar
  19. Fraisse, P (1978). Time and rhythm perception. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.),Handbook of perception. Vol. VIII: Perceptual coding (pp. 203–254). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  20. Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time.Annual Review of Psychology,35, 1–36.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Frankenhaeuser, M. (1959).Estimation of time. Uppsala, Sweden: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
  22. Gilliland, A. R., Hofeld, J., &Eckstrand, G. (1946). Studies in time perception.Psychological Bulletin,43, 162–176.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Goldstone, S (1975). The variability of temporal judgment in psychopathology. In M. L. Kietzman, S. Sutton, & J. Zubin (Eds.),Experimental approaches to psychopathology (pp. 393–419). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  24. Guay, M. (1982). Long-term retention of temporal information.Perceptual & Motor Skills,54, 843–849.Google Scholar
  25. Gulliksen, H. (1927). The influence of occupation upon the perception of time.Journal of Experimental Psychology,10, 52–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Harton, J. J. (1938a). The influence of the difficulty of activity on the estimation of time.Journal of Experimental Psychology,23, 270–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Harton, J. J. (1938b). The influence of the difficulty of activity on the estimation of time: II.Journal of Experimental Psychology,23, 428–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Harton, J. J. (1942). Time estimation in relation to goal organization and difficulty of tasks.Journal of General Psychology,27, 63–69.Google Scholar
  29. Hawkes, G. R. (1972). Time perception research: An empirical basis for the study of attention.Psychonomic Monographs Supplement,4(12, Whole No. 60), 221–224.Google Scholar
  30. Hawkes, G. R., &Sherman, S. J. (1972). Vigilance effects for duration judgments with two levels of task demand.Perceptual & Motor Skills,34, 351–356.Google Scholar
  31. Hicks, R. E., &Brundige, R. M. (1974). Judgments of temporal duration while processing verbal and physiognomic stimuli.Acta Psychologica,38, 447–453.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Hicks, R. E., &Miller, G. W. (1976). Transfer of time judgments as a function of feedback.American Journal of Psychology,89, 303–310.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Hicks, R. E., Miller, G. W., Gaes, G., &Bierman, K. (1977). Concurrent processing demands and the experience of time-in-passing.American Journal of Psychology,90, 431–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hicks, R. E., Miller, G. W., &Kinsbourne, M. (1976). Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processed.American Journal of Psychology,89, 719–730.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Hornstein, A. D., &Rotter, G. S. (1969). Research methodology in temporal perception.Journal of Experimental Psychology,79, 561–564.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. James, W. (1950).The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1890)Google Scholar
  37. Kahneman, D. (1975). Effort, recognition and recall in auditory attention. In P. Rabbitt & S. Domic (Eds.),Attention and performance V (pp. 65–80). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  38. Keppel, G. (1982).Design and analysis: A researchers handbook (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  39. Loehlin, J. C. (1959). The influence of different activities on the apparent length of time.Psychological Monographs,73(4, Whole No. 474).Google Scholar
  40. Martin, D. W. (1970). Residual processing capacity during verbal organization in memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,9, 391–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Mcclain, L. (1983). Interval estimation: Effects of processing demands on prospective and retrospective reports.Perception & Psychophysics,34, 185–189.Google Scholar
  42. Mcconchie, R. D., &Rutschmann, J. (1971). Human time estimation On differences between methods.Perceptual & Motor Skills,32, 319–336.Google Scholar
  43. Mckay, T. D. (1977). Time estimation: Effects of attentional focus and a comparison of interval conditions.Perceptual & Motor Skills,45, 584–586.Google Scholar
  44. Michon, J. A (1972). Processing of temporal information and the cognitive theory of time experience. In J. T. Fraser, F. C. Haber, & G. W. Muller (Eds.),The study of time (pp. 242–258). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  45. Miller, G. W., Hicks, R. E., &Willette, M. (1978). Effects of concurrent verbal rehearsal and temporal set upon judgments of temporal duration.Acta Psychologica,42, 173–179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Morton, J., Marcus, S., &Frankish, C. (1976). Perceptual centers (P-centers).Psychological Review,83, 405–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Ornstein, R. E (1969).On the experience of time. Baltimore: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  48. Schwartz, W. (1978). Time and context during hypnotic involvement.International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,26, 307–316.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Smith, N. C. (1969) The effects on time estimation of increasing the complexity of a cognitive taskJournal of General Psychology,81, 231–235.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Sturt, M. (1925).The psychology of time. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  51. Thomas, E. A. C, &Brown, I. (1974). Time perception and the filled-duration illusion.Perception & Psychophysics,16, 449–458Google Scholar
  52. Thomas, E. A. C, &Cantor, N. E. (1978) Interdependence between the processing of temporal and non-temporal information In J Raquin (Ed.),Attention and performance VIII (pp. 43–62). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  53. Thomas, E. A. C, &Weaver, W. B. (1975). Cognitive processing and time perception.Perception & Psychophysics,17, 363–367.Google Scholar
  54. Toglia, M, &Battig, W. (1978).Handbook of semantic word norms Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
  55. Trumbo, S., &Milone, F. (1971). Primary task performance as a function of encoding, retention, and recall in a secondary task.Journal of Experimental Psychology,91, 271–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Underwood, G. (1975). Attention and the perception of duration during encoding and retrieval.Perception,4, 291–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Underwood, G., &Swain, R. A. (1973). Selectivity of attention and the perception of durationPerception,2, 101–105.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Vroon, P. A. (1970). Effects of presented and processed information on duration experience.Acta Psychologica,24, 115–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wachtel, P. L. (1967). Conceptions of broad and narrow attentionPsychological Bulletin,68, 417–429.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Wilsoncroft, W. E., &Stone, J D. (1975). Information processing and estimation of short time intervals.Perceptual & Motor Skills,41, 192–194Google Scholar
  61. Zakay, D., Nitzan, D., &Glicksohn, J. (1983) The influence of task difficulty and external tempo on subjective time estimation.Perception & Psychophysics,34, 451–456Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Scott W. Brown
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MaineOrono

Personalised recommendations