Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 6, Issue 5, pp 568–581 | Cite as

From print to sound in mature readers as a function of reader ability and two forms of orthographic regularity

  • Mildred Mason
Article

Abstract

Four experiments using college students as subjects provide evidence that both highly skilled and less skilled mature readers derive the names of printed words from visual access of the lexicon rather than by phonological recoding. Regularity of pronunciation (regular vs. exception words) as a variable of orthographic regularity effectiveafter visual code formation had no effect either between or within reading ability groups. Less skilled readers made more errors and were slower than highly skilled readers on both types of words. Sing-letter spatial redundancy, as a variable of orthographic regularity that influences the formation of visual codes, served to differentiate the two groups only in naming nonwords. Highly skilled readers used spatial redundancy to offset the effect of array length, whereas less skilled readers did not. Except for high-frequency words, visual access and retrieval of the pronunciation of words was significantly faster for highly skilled readers. Less skilled readers were most disadvantaged in naming nonwords, a task that requires phonological recoding. Overall results support the hypothesis that reading ability in mature readers is related to the speed of word recognition. Highly skilled readers may make more use of variables of orthographic regularity effective in the formation of visual codes.

Keywords

Serial Position Skilled Reader Reading Ability Regular Word Visual Code 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Reference Notes

  1. 1. Frederiksen, J. R.Decoding skills and lexical retrieval. Paper presented at the Psychonomic Society meeting, St. Louis, 1976.Google Scholar
  2. 2. Mason, M.Perception of the order of letters and nonsense forms in fifth-grade children as a function of reader ability. Unpublished manuscript, 1975. (Available from M. Mason, Iowa State University. Ames, Iowa 50011.)Google Scholar

References

  1. Baron, J.. &Strawson.C. Use of orthographic and wordspecific knowledge in reading words aloud.Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 1976,2, 386–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baron, J., &Thurston, I. An analysis of the word-superiority effect.Cognitive Psychology, 1973,4, 207–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Coltheart, M. Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.),Strategies of information processing. New York: Academic Press, in press.Google Scholar
  4. Corkin, S. Serial-ordering deficits in inferior readers.Neuropsychologia, 1974,12, 347–354.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Forster, K. I., &Chambers.S. M. Lexical access and naming time.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1973.12, 627–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Frederiksen, J. R.. &Kroll.J. F. Spelling to sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon.Journal of Experiment Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 1976,2, 361–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gounkoff, R. M., &Rosinski.R. R. Decoding, semantic processing and reading comprehension skill.Child Development, 1976,47, 252–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Groff, P. Research In brief: Shapes as cues to word recognition.Visible Language, 1975,9, 67–71.Google Scholar
  9. Kolers, P. A. Three stages of reading. In H. Levin & J. P. Williams (Eds.),Basic studies on reading. New York: Basic Books, 1970.Google Scholar
  10. Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A. M..Fowler, C., &Fischer, F. W. Phonetic segmentation and recoding In the beginning reader. In A. S. Reber & D. L. Scarborough (Eds.),Toward a psychology of reading. The proceedings of the CUNY Conference. New York: John Wiley. 1977.Google Scholar
  11. Makita, K. Rarity of reading disability in Japanese children.American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1968,38, 599–614.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Manelis, L. The effect of meaningfulness on tachistoscopic word perception.Perception & Psychophysics, 1974,16, 182–192.Google Scholar
  13. Mason.M. Reading ability and letter search time: Effects of orthographic structure defined by single-letter positional frequency.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1975.104. 146–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Mason. M. The role of spatial redundancy in grapheme recognition: Perception or inference?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, in press.Google Scholar
  15. Mason.M.. &Katz.L. Visual processing of nonlinguistic strings: Redundancy effects and reading ability.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1976,105, 338–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Mason.M., Katz.L., &Wicklund, D. A. Immediate spatial order memory and item memory in sixth-grade children as a function of reader ability.Journal of Educational Psychology, 1975,67, 610–616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Massaro, D. W., Venezky, R. L., & Taylor. G. A. Orthographic regularity, positional frequency, and visual processing of letter strings.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, in press.Google Scholar
  18. Mattingly.I. G. Reading, the linguistic process, and linguistic awareness. In J. F. Kavanaugh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.),Language by ear and by eye. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. 1972.Google Scholar
  19. Mayzner, M. S., & Tresselt. M. E. Tables of single-letter and diagram frequency counts for various word-length and letter-position combinations.Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1965,l(Whole No.2). 13-32.Google Scholar
  20. McClelland.J. L. Preliminary letter identification in the perception of words and nonwords.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976,2, 80–91.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. McClelland, J. L., &Johnston, J. C. The role of familiar units in the perception of words and nonwords.Perception & Psychophysics, 1977.22, 249–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Pachella.R. G. The interpretation of reaction time in information-processing research. In B. H. Kantowitz (Ed.),Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition. New York: John Wiley. 1974.Google Scholar
  23. Perfetti.C. A., &Hogaboam.T. Relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension skill.Journal of Educational Psychology, 1975.67.461–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rozin.P., &Gleitman.L. R. The structure and acquisition of readingII: The reading process and the acquisition of the alphabetic principle. In A. S. Reber & D. L. Scarborough (Eds.),Toward a psychology of reading: The proceedings of the CUNY conference. New York: John Wiley, 1977.Google Scholar
  25. Rozin, P., Poritsky, S., &Sotsby, R. American children with reading problems can easily learn to read English represented by Chinese characters.Science. 1971.171, 1264–1267.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Rubenstein.H., Lewis, S. S., &Rubenstein, M. A. Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971,10. 645–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Smith, E. E., &Spoehr, K. T. The perception of printed English: A theoretical perspective. In B. H. Kantowitz (Ed.),Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition. New York: John Wiley, 1974.Google Scholar
  28. Smith.F.Understanding reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart. & Winston, 1971.Google Scholar
  29. Thorndike.E. L., &Lorge.L.The teacher’s word book of 30,000 words. New York: Teacher’s College, Columbia University Press, 1944.Google Scholar
  30. Venezky.R. L.The structure of English orthography. The Hague: Mouton, 1970.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1978

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mildred Mason
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyIowa State UniversityAmes

Personalised recommendations