Memory & Cognition

, Volume 11, Issue 3, pp 295–306

On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning

  • J. St. B. T. Evans
  • Julie L. Barston
  • Paul Pollard
Open Access
Article

Abstract

Three experiments are reported that investigate the weighting attached to logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Substantial belief biases were observed despite controls for possible conversions of the premises. Equally substantial effects of logic were observed despite controls for two possible response biases. A consistent interaction between belief and logic was also recorded; belief bias was more marked on invalid than on valid syllogisms. In all experiments, verbal protocols were recorded and analyzed. These protocols are interpreted in some cases as providing rationalizations for prejudiced decisions and, in other cases, as reflecting a genuine process of premise to conclusion reasoning. In the latter cases, belief bias was minimal but still present. Similarly, even subjects who focus primarily on the conclusion are influenced to an extent by the logic. Thus a conflict between logic and belief is observed throughout, but at several levels of extent.

References

  1. Begg, I., &Denny, J. P. Empirical reconciliation of atmosphere and conversion interpretations of syllogistic reasoning errors.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969,111, 351–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Braine, M. D. S. On the relation between the natural logic of reasoning and standard logic.Psychological Review, 1978,85, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cohen, L. J. Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated?Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 1981,4, 317–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Dickstein, L. S. The effect of figure on syllogistic reasoning.Memory & Cognition, 1978,6, 76–83. (a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dickstein, L. S. Error processes in syllogistic reasoning.Memory & Cognition, 1978,6, 537–543. (b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dickstein, L. S. Inference errors in deductive reasoning.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1980,6, 414–416.Google Scholar
  7. Dickstein, L. S. Conversion and possibility in syllogistic reasoning.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1981,18, 229–232.Google Scholar
  8. Erickson, J. A set analysis theory of behavior in formal syllogistic reasoning tasks. In R. L. Solso (Ed.),Theories of cognitive psychology: The Loyola Symposium. Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum, 1974.Google Scholar
  9. Ericsson, K. A., &Simon, H. A. Verbal reports as data.Psychological Review, 1980,117, 215–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Evans, J. St. B. T. Linguistic factors in reasoning.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1977,29, 297–306. (a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Evans, J. St. B. T. Toward a statistical theory of reasoningQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1977,29, 621–635. (b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Evans, J. St. B. T. The psychology of deductive reasoning. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1982.Google Scholar
  13. Falmagne, R. J. (Ed.),Reasoning: Representation and process. Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum, 1975.Google Scholar
  14. Feather, N. T. Acceptance and rejection of arguments in relation to attitude strength, critical ability and intolerance of inconsistency.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1964,69, 127–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gorden, R. Attitudes toward Russia on logical reasoning.Journal of Social Psychology, 1953,37, 103–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Guyote, M. J., &Sternberg, R. J. A transitive-chain theory of syllogistic reasoning.Cognitive Psychology, 1981,13, 461–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Henle, M. On the relation between logic and thinking.Psychological Review, 1962,69, 366–378.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Henle, M., &Michael, M. The influence of attitudes on syllogistic reasoning.Journal of Social Psychology, 1956,44, 115–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Inhelder, B., &Piaget, J. The growth of logical thinking. New York: Basic Books, 1958.Google Scholar
  20. Janis, I., &Frick, P. The relationship between attitudes toward conclusions and errors in judging logical validity of syllogisms.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1943,33, 73–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Janis, I., &Terwilliger, R. An experimental study of psychological resistances to fear arousing communications.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962,65, 403–410.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Johnson-Laird, P., &Steedman, M. The psychology of syllogisms.Cognitive Psychology, 1978,10, 64–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kaufman, H., &Goldstein, S. The effects of emotional value of conclusions upon distortion in syllogistic reasoning.Psychonomic Science, 1967,7, 367–368.Google Scholar
  24. Lefford, A. The influence of emotional subject matter on logical reasoning.Journal of General Psychology, 1946,34, 127–151.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lord, C., Lepper, M. R., & Ross, L. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effect of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1979,37.Google Scholar
  26. Morgan, J., &Morton, J. The distortion of syllogistic reasoning produced by personal convictions.Journal of Social Psychology, 1944,20, 39–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Nisbett, R. E., &Ross, L. Human inference: Strategies and short-comings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice-Hall, 1980.Google Scholar
  28. Nisbett, R. E., &Wilson, T. D. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.Psychological Review, 1977,14, 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Pollard, P. Human reasoning: Logical and nonlogical explanations. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Plymouth Polytechnic, 1979.Google Scholar
  30. Pollard, P. Human reasoning: Some possible effects of availability.Cognition, 1982,12, 65–96.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Revlin, R. Syllogistic reasoning: Logical decisions from a complex data base. In R. Falmagne (Ed.),Reasoning: Representation and process. Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum, 1975. (a)Google Scholar
  32. Revlin, R. Two models of syIlogistic reasoning: Feature selection and conversion.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1975,14, 180–195. (b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Revlin, R., &Leirer, V. The effects of personal biases on syllogistic reasoning: Rational decisions from personalized representations. In R. Revlin & R. E. Mayer (Eds.),Human reasoning. Washington, D.C: Winston-Wiley, 1978.Google Scholar
  34. Revlin, R., Leirer, V., Yopp, H., &Yopp, R. The belief-bias effect in formal reasoning: The influence of knowledge on logic.Memory & Cognition, 1980,8, 584–592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Revlin, R., &Mayer, R. E. (Eds.).Human reasoning. Washington, D.C: Winston-Wiley, 1978.Google Scholar
  36. Siegel, S. Nonparametric statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956.Google Scholar
  37. Wason, P. C., &Evans, J. St. B. T. Dual processes in reasoning?Cognition, 1975,3, 141–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wilkins, M. The effect of changed material on ability to do formal syllogistic reasoning.Archives of Psychology, 1928,16, 83.Google Scholar
  39. Wilson, W. The effect of competition on the speed and accuracy of syllogistic reasoning.Journal of Social Psychology, 1965,65, 27–32.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Winthrop, H. Semantic factors in the measurement of personality integration.Journal of Social Psychology, 1946,14, 149–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Woodworth, R. S., &Sells, S. B. An atmosphere effect in syllogistic reasoning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1935,18, 451–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. St. B. T. Evans
    • 1
  • Julie L. Barston
    • 1
  • Paul Pollard
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyPlymouth PolytechnicPlymouthEngland

Personalised recommendations