Memory & Cognition

, Volume 29, Issue 3, pp 462–477 | Cite as

The representations of the arithmetic operations include functional relationships

Article

Abstract

Current theories of mathematical problem solving propose that people select a mathematical operation as the solution to a problem on the basis of a structure mapping between their problem representation and the representation of the mathematical operations. The structure-mapping hypothesis requires that the problem and the mathematical representations contain analogous relations. Past research has demonstrated that the problem representation consists of functional relationships, orprinciples. The present study tested whether people represent analogous principles for each arithmetic operation (i.e., addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division). For each operation, college (Experiments 1 and 2) and 8th grade (Experiment 2) participants were asked to rate the degree to which a set of completed problems was a good attempt at the operation. The pattern of presented answers either violated one of four principles or did not violate any principles. The distance of the presented answers from the correct answers was independently manipulated. Consistent with the hypothesis that people represent the principles, (1) violations of the principles were rated as poorer attempts at the operation, (2) operations that are learned first (e.g., addition) had more extensive principle representations than did operations learned later (multiplication), and (3) principles that are more frequently in evidence developed more quickly. In Experiment 3, college participants rated the degree to which statements were indicative of each operation. The statements were either consistent or inconsistent with one of two principles. The participants’ ratings showed that operations with longer developmental histories had strong principle representations. The implications for a structure-mapping approach to mathematical problem solving are discussed.

References

  1. Bassok, M., Chase, V.M., &Martin, S.A. (1998). Adding apples and oranges: Alignment of semantic and formal knowledge.Cognitive Psychology,35, 99–134.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Campbell, J. I. D. (1999). Division by multiplication.Memory & Cognition,27, 791–802.Google Scholar
  3. Dixon, J. A., &Moore, C. F. (1996). The developmental role of intuitive principles in choosing mathematical strategies.Developmental Psychology,32, 241–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Dixon, J. A., &Moore, C. F. (1997). Characterizing the intuitive representation in problem solving: Evidence from evaluating mathematical strategies.Memory & Cognition,25, 395–412.Google Scholar
  5. Dixon, J. A., &Tuccillo, F. (2001). Generating initial models for reasoning.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,78, 178–212.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Geary, D. C. (1994).Children’s mathematical development: Research and practical applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction.American Psychologist,50, 24–37.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy.Cognitive Science,7, 155–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gentner, D., &Markman, A. B. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity.American Psychologist,52, 45–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hardiman, P. T., Dufresne, R., &Mestre, J. P. (1989). The relation between problem categorization and problem solving among experts and novices.Memory & Cognition,17, 627–638.Google Scholar
  11. Kintsch, W., &Greeno, J.G. (1985). Understanding and solving word arithmetic problems.Psychological Review,92, 109–129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Krueger, L.E. (1986). Why 2 32 = 5 looks so wrong: On the odd-even rule in product verification.Memory & Cognition,14, 141–149.Google Scholar
  13. Krueger, L. E., &Hallford, E. W. (1984). Why 2 + 2 = 5 looks so wrong: On the odd-even rule in sum verification.Memory & Cognition,12, 171–180.Google Scholar
  14. Lemaire, P., &Reder, L. (1999). What affects strategy selection in arithmetic? The example of parity and five effects on product verification.Memory & Cognition,27, 364–382.Google Scholar
  15. Levin, I. P., Johnson, R.D., &Faraone, S.V. (1984). Information integration in price-quality tradeoffs: The effect of missing information.Memory & Cognition,12, 96–102.Google Scholar
  16. Miller, K., Perlmutter, M., &Keating, D. (1984). Cognitive arithmetic: Comparison of operations.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,10, 46–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Moore, C. F., Dixon, J.A., &Haines, B.A. (1991). Components of understanding in proportional reasoning: A fuzzy set representation of developmental progression.Child Development,62, 441–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989).Curriculum and evaluation standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.Google Scholar
  19. Nesher, P. (1982). Levels of description in the analysis of addition and subtraction word problems. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.),Addition and subtraction: A cognitive perspective (pp. 25–38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  20. Novick, L. R., &Holyoak, K. J. (1991). Mathematical problem solving by analogy.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 398–415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Reese, C. M., Miller, K. E., Mazzeo, J., &Dossey, J. A. (1997).NAEP 1996 mathematics report card for the nation and the states. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.Google Scholar
  22. Rittle-Johnson, B., &Siegler, R. S. (1998). The relation between conceptual and procedural knowledge in learning mathematics: A review. In C. Donlan (Ed.),The development of mathematical skills: Studies in developmental psychology (pp. 75–110). Hove, U.K.: Taylor & Francis, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  23. Ross, B. H., &Kilbane, M. C. (1997). Effects of principle explanation and superficial similarity on analogical mapping in problem solving.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 427–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Schneider, W., Gruber, H., Gold, A., &Opwis, K. (1993). Chess expertise and memory for chess positions in children and adults.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,56, 328–349.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Stevens, S. S. (1974). Perceptual magnitude and its measurement. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.),Handbook of Perception: Vol. II. Psychophysical judgment and measurement (pp. 361–389). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  26. Surber, C. F. (1980). The development of reversible operations in judgments of ability, effort, and performance.Child Development,51, 1018–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • James A. Dixon
    • 1
  • Julie K. Deets
    • 2
  • Ashley Bangert
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCollege of William and MaryWilliamsburg
  2. 2.Trinity UniversitySan Antonio

Personalised recommendations