Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words

Article

Abstract

Age of acquisition and imageability ratings were collected for 2,645 words, including 892 verbs and 213 function words. Words that were ambiguous as to grammatical category were disambiguated: Verbs were shown in their infinitival form, and nouns (where appropriate) were preceded by the indefinite article (such asto crack anda crack). Subjects were speakers of British English selected from a wide age range, so that differences in the responses across age groups could be compared. Within the subset of early acquired noun/verb homonyms, the verb forms were rated as later acquired than the nouns, and the verb homonyms of high-image ability nouns were rated as significantly less imageable than their noun counterparts. A small number of words received significantly earlier or later age of acquisition ratings when the 20–40 years and 50–80 years age groups were compared. These tend to comprise words that have come to be used more frequently in recent years (either through technological advances or social change), or those that have fallen out of common usage. Regression analyses showed that although word length, familiarity, and concreteness make independent contributions to the age of acquisition measure, frequency and imageability are the most important predictors of rated age of acquisition.

Supplementary material

Bird-BRM-2001.zip (269 kb)
Supplementary material, approximately 340 KB.

References

  1. Allport, D. A., &Funnell, E. (1981). Components of the mental lexicon.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B,295, 397–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baayen, R.H., Piepenbrock, R., &Gulikers, L. (1995).The CELEX lexical database [CD-ROM], Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.Google Scholar
  3. Barry, C., Morrison, C. M., &Ellis, A. W. (1997). Naming the Snod-grass and Vanderwart pictures: Effects of age of acquisition, frequency and name agreement.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,50A, 560–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bastiaanse, R., &Jonkers, R. (1998). Verb retrieval in action naming and spontaneous speech in agrammatic and anomic aphasia.Aphasiology,12, 951–969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D., Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J. S., Reilly, J., &Hartung, J. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary.Journal of Child Language,21, 85–123.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Berndt, R. S., Mitchum, C. C., Haendiges, A. N., &Sandson, J. (1997). Verb retrieval in aphasia: 1. Characterizing single word impairments.Brain & Language,56, 68–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bird, H. (1999).Processing categories of vocabulary in aphasia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Newcastle upon Tyne.Google Scholar
  8. Caramazza, A., &Hillis, A.E. (1991). Lexical organization of nouns and verbs in the brain.Nature,349, 788–790.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carroll, J. B., &White, M.N. (1973). Word frequency and age of acquisition as determiners of picture-naming latency.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,25, 85–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Caselli, M. C., Bates, E., Casadio, P., Fenson, J., Fenson, L., Sanders, L., &Weir, J. (1995). A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development.Cognitive Development,10, 159–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Coltheart, M. (1981a). The MRC psycholinguistic database.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,33A, 497–505.Google Scholar
  12. Coltheart, M. (1981b).MRC psycholinguistic database user manual: Version 1.Google Scholar
  13. Davelaar, E., &Besner, D. (1988). Word identification: Imageability, semantics, and the content-functor distinction.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,40A, 789–799.Google Scholar
  14. DeGroot, A. M. B. (1989). Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,15, 824–845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Denes, G., &Dalla Barba, G. (1998). G. B. Vico, precursor of cognitive neuropsychology? The first reported case of noun-verb dissociation following brain damage.Brain & Language,62, 29–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Forster, K. I., &Chambers, I. M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,12, 627–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Franklin, S., Howard, D., &Patterson, K. (1994). Abstract word meaning deafness.Cognitive Neuropsychology,2, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Franklin, S., Howard, D., &Patterson, K. (1995). Abstract word anomia.Cognitive Neuropsychology,12, 549–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Friederici, A. D., &Schoenle, P. W. (1980). Computational dissociation of two vocabulary types: Evidence from aphasia.Neuropsychologia,18, 11–20.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. I. Kuczaj (Ed.),Language development: Language, thought and culture (pp. 301–334). Hills-dale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Gilhooly, K. J., &Hay, D. (1977). Imagery, concreteness, age-of-acquisition, familiarity, and meaningfulness value for 205 five-letter words having single-solutionanagrams.Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation,9, 12–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gilhooly, K. J., &Logie, R. H. (1980). Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words.Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation,12, 395–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gilhooly, K. J., &Logie, R. H. (1981). Word age-of-acquisition and visual recognition thresholds.Current Psychological Research,1, 215–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hanley, R. J., &Kay, J. (1997). An effect of imageability on the production of phonological errors in auditory repetition.Cognitive Neuro-psychology,14, 1065–1084.Google Scholar
  25. Hillis, A. E., &Caramazza, A. (1995). Representation of grammatical categories of words in thebrain.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,7, 396–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Howard, D., Best, W., Bruce, C., &Gatehouse, C. (1995). Operativity and animacy effects in aphasic naming.European Journal of Disorders of Communication,30, 286–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Howard, D., &Franklin, S. (1988).Missing the meaning? A cognitive neuropsychological study of the processing of words by an aphasic patient. London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  28. Howard, D., Patterson, K., Franklin, S., Morton, J., &Orchard-Lisle, V. (1984). Variability and consistency in picture naming by aphasie patients. In F. C. Rose (Ed.),Advances in neurology: Vol. 42. Progress in aphasiology (pp. 263–276). New York: Raven.Google Scholar
  29. James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,1, 130–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Jones, G. V. (1985). Deep dyslexia, imageability and ease of predication.Brain & Language,24, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kay, J., &Ellis, A. W. (1987). A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia: Implications for psychological models of word retrieval.Brain,110, 613–629.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Klee, H., &Legge, D. (1976). Estimates of concreteness and other indices for 200 transitive verbs.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,2, 497–507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lambon Ralph, M. A., Graham, K. S., Ellis, A. W., &Hodges, J. (1998). Naming in semantic dementia: What matters?Neuropsychologia,36, 775–784.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Marcel, A. J., &Patterson, K. (1978). Word recognition and production: Reciprocity in clinical and normal studies. In J. Requin (Ed.),Attention and performance VII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  35. Masterson, J., &Druks, J. (1998). Description of a set of 164 nouns and 102 verbs matched for printed word frequency, familiarity and age-of-acquisition.Journal of Neurolinguistics,11, 331–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Molfese, D. L., Burger-Judisch, L. M., Gill, L. A., Golinkoff, R. M., &Hirsch-Pasek, K. A. (1996). Electrophy siological correlates of noun-verb processing in adults.Brain & Language,54, 388–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., &Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,50A, 528–559.Google Scholar
  38. Morrison, C. M., &Ellis, A.W. (1995). The roles of word frequency and age of acquisition in word naming and lexical decision.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 116–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Morrison, C. M., Ellis, A. W., &Quinlan, P. T. (1992). Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition.Memory & Cognition,20, 705–714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Morton, J., &Patterson, K. (1980). “Little words—No!” In M. Coltheart, K. Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.),Deep dyslexia (pp. 270–285). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  41. Nickels, L., &Howard, D. (1995). Aphasie naming—What matters?Neuropsychologia,33, 1281–1303.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Oldfield, R. C., &Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,17, 273–281.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Orpwood, L., &Warrington, E. K. (1995). Word specific impairments in naming and spelling but not in reading.Cortex,31, 239–265.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., &Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns.Journal of Experimental Psychology,76, 1–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Plaut, D. C., &Shallice, T. (1993). Deep dyslexia—A case study of connectionist neuropsychology.Cognitive Neuropsychology,10, 377–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Pulvermüller, F., Mohr, B., Sedat, N., Hadler, B., &Rayman, J. (1996). Word class-specific deficits in Wernicke’s aphasia.Neuro-case,2, 203–212.Google Scholar
  47. Rapp, B., &Caramazza, A. (1998). A case of selective difficulty in writing verbs.Neurocase,4, 127–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Richardson, J. T. E. (1975). The effect of word imageability in acquired dyslexia.Neuropsychologia,13, 281–288.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Shallice, T. (1988).From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Strain, E., Patterson, K., &Seidenberg, M. S. (1995). Semantic effects in single word naming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 1140–1154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. ter Keurs, M., Brown, C. M., Hagoort, P., &Stegeman, D. F. (1999). Electrophysical manifestations of open- and closed-class words in patients with Broca’s aphasia with agrammatic comprehension.Brain,122, 839–854.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Warburton, E., Wise, R.J.S., Price, C. J., Weiller, C., Hadar, U., Ramsay, S., &Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1996). Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects: Studies with PET.Brain,119, 159–179.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of Newcastle upon TyneNewcastle upon TyneEngland
  2. 2.MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences UnitCambridgeEngland

Personalised recommendations