Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 14, Issue 3, pp 436–441 | Cite as

Emotion simulation during language comprehension

  • David A. Havas
  • Arthur M. Glenberg
  • Mike Rinck
Brief Reports

Abstract

We report a novel finding on the relation of emotion and language. Covert manipulation of emotional facial posture interacts with sentence valence when measuring the amount of time to judge valence (Experiment 1) and sensibility (Experiment 2) of the sentence. In each case, an emotion-sentence compatibility effect is found: Judgment times are faster when facial posture and sentence valence match than when they mismatch. We interpret the finding using a simulation account; that is, emotional systems contribute to language comprehension much as they do in social interaction. Because the effect was not observed on a lexical decision task using emotion-laden words (Experiment 3), we suggest that the emotion simulation affects comprehension processes beyond initial lexical access.

Keywords

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Target Word Lexical Decision Lexical Decision Task Language Comprehension 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds?Perspectives on Psychological Science,1, 28–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems.Behavioral & Brain Sciences,22, 577–660.Google Scholar
  3. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist,36, 129–148.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bower, G. H. (1991). Mood congruity of social judgments. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.),Emotion and social judgments (pp. 31–53). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  5. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999).Affective norms for English words (ANEW). Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention.Google Scholar
  6. Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V., &Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: A combined TMS and behavioral study.Cognitive Brain Research,24, 355–363.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M., Mazziotta, J. C., &Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,100, 5497–5502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chambers, C. G., Tanenhaus, M. K., &Magnuson, J. S. (2004). Actions and affordances in syntactic ambiguity resolution.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,30, 687–696.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chen, M., &Bargh, J. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus.Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin,25, 215–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Davidson, R. J. (1994). Complexities in the search for emotion-specific physiology. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.),The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 237–242). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Decety, J., &Grèzes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior.Brain Research,1079, 4–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. de Gelder, B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language.Nature Reviews Neuroscience,7, 242–249.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to facial expressions.Psychophysiology,19, 643–647.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., &Elmehed, K. (2000). Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions.Psychological Science,11, 86–89.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM).Psychological Bulletin,117, 39–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Frijda, N. H. (1986).The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Gibson, J. J. (1979).The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  18. Glenberg, A. M., &Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,9, 558–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Glenberg, A. M., &Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions.Discourse Processes,28, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., &Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex.Neuron,41, 301–307.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Innes-Ker, Å. [H.], &Niedenthal, P. M. (2002). Emotion concepts and emotional states in social judgment and categorization.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,83, 804–816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Johnson, E. J., &Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,45,20–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kaschak, M. P., &Glenberg, A. M. (2000). Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension.Journal of Memory & Language,43, 508–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A., &Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing.Cognition,94, B79-B89.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Larsen, R. J., Mercer, K. A., &Balota, D. A. (2006). Lexical characteristics of words used in emotional Stroop experiments.Emotion,6, 62–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Levenson, R. W. (2003). Blood, sweat, and fears: The autonomic architecture of emotion.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1000, 348–366.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., &Freisen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity.Psychophysiology,27, 363–384.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. McIntosh, D. N., Zajonc, R. B., Vig, P. S., &Emerick, S. W. (1997). Facial movement, breathing, temperature, and affect: Implications of the vascular theory of emotional efference.Cognition & Emotion,11, 171–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Halberstadt, J. B., &Innes-Ker, Å. H. (2001). When did her smile drop? Facial mimicry and the influences of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression.Cognition & Emotion,15, 853–864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Niedenthal, P. M., Halberstadt, J. B., &Setterlund, M. B. (1997). Being happy and seeing “happy”: Emotional state mediates visual word recognition.Cognition & Emotion,11, 403–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Oatley, K. (1994). A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative.Poetics,23, 53–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Raaijmakers, J. G. W., Schrijnemakers, J. M. C., &Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with “The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy”: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions.Journal of Memory & Language,41, 416–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., &Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action.Nature Reviews Neuroscience,2, 661–670.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Strack, F., Martin, L. L., &Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,54, 768–777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,17, 271–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states.Behaviour Research & Therapy,6, 473–482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Zwaan, R. A., &Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,135, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • David A. Havas
    • 2
  • Arthur M. Glenberg
    • 2
  • Mike Rinck
    • 1
  1. 1.Dresden University of TechnologyDresdenGermany
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WisconsinMadison

Personalised recommendations