Memory & Cognition

, Volume 35, Issue 2, pp 242–253 | Cite as

Learning lyrics: To sing or not to sing?

  • Amélie Racette
  • Isabelle Peretz
Article
  • 1.9k Downloads

Abstract

According to common practice and oral tradition, learning verbal materials through song should facilitate word recall. In the present study, we provide evidence against this belief. In Experiment 1, 36 university students, half of them musicians, learned an unfamiliar song in three conditions. In the sung—sung condition, the song to be learned was sung, and the response was sung too. In the sung—spoken condition, the response was spoken. In the divided—spoken condition, the presented lyrics (accompanied by music) and the response were both spoken. Superior word recall in the sung—sung condition was predicted. However, fewer words were recalled when singing than when speaking. Furthermore, the mode of presentation, whether sung or spoken, had no influence on lyric recall, in either short- or long-term recall. In Experiment 2, singing was assessed with and without words. Altogether, the results indicate that the text and the melody of a song have separate representations in memory, making singing a dual task to perform, at least in the first steps of learning. Interestingly, musical training had little impact on performance, suggesting that vocal learning is a basic and widespread skill.

References

  1. Besson, M., Faïta, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A.-M., &Requin, J. (1998). Singing in the brain: Independence of lyrics and tunes.Psychological Science,9, 494–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bigand, E., &Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). Are we experienced listeners? A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal musical training.Cognition,100, 100–130.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bonnel, A.-M., Faïta, F., Peretz, I., &Besson, M. (2001). Divided attention between lyrics and tunes of operatic songs: Evidence for independent processing.Perception & Psychophysics,63, 1201–1213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Booth, M. W. (1981).The experience of songs. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bowra, C. M. (1962). Primitive song. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
  6. Calvert, S. L., &Billingsley, R. L. (1998). Young children’s recitation and comprehension of information presented by songs.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,19, 97–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Calvert, S. L., &Tart, M. (1993). Song versus verbal forms for verylong-term, long-term, and short-term verbatim recall.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,14, 245–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chan, A. S., Ho, Y.-C., &Cheung, M.-C. (1998). Music training improves verbal memory.Nature,396, 128.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Chazin, S., &Neuschatz, J. S. (1990). Using a mnemonic to aid in the recall of unfamiliar information.Perceptual & Motor Skills,71, 1067–1071.Google Scholar
  10. Crowder, R. G., Serafine, M. L., &Repp, B. (1990). Physical interaction and association by contiguity in memory for the words and melodies of songs.Memory & Cognition,18, 469–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dell, F. (1989). Concordances rythmiques entre la musique et les paroles dans le chant: L’accent de l’e muet dans la chanson française. In M. Dominicy (Ed.),Le souci des apparences (pp. 121–136). Bruxelles: Université de Bruxelles.Google Scholar
  12. Dickson, D., &Grant, L. (2003). Physics karaoke: Why not?Physics Education,4, 320–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Drake, C., &Palmer, C. (2000). Skill acquisition in music performance: Relations between planning and temporal control.Cognition,74, 1–32.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Gingold, H., &Abravanel, E. (1987). Music as a mnemonic: The effects of good- and bad-music settings on verbatim recall of short passages by young children.Psychomusicology,7, 25–39.Google Scholar
  15. Ginsborg, J. (2002). Classical singers learning and memorising a new song: An observational study.Psychology of Music,30, 58–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hayes, N., &Kaun, A. (1996). The role of phonological phrasing in sung and chanted verse.Linguistic Review,13, 243–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hébert, S., &Peretz, I. (2001). Are text and tune of familiar songs separable by brain damage?Brain & Cognition,46, 169–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hébert, S., Racette, A., Gagnon, L., &Peretz, I. (2003). Revisiting the dissociation between singing and speaking in expressive aphasia.Brain,126, 1838–1850.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Ho, Y.-C., Cheung, M.-C., &Chan, A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children.Neuropsychology,17, 439–450.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Jakobson, L. S., Cuddy, L. L., &Kilgour, A. R. (2003). Time tagging: A key to musicians’ superior memory.Music Perception,20, 307–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Janik, V. M., &Slater, P. J. B. (1997). Vocal learning in mammals.Advances in the Study of Behavior,26, 56–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jellison, J. A., &Miller, N. L. (1982). Recall of digit and word sequences by musicians and nonmusicians as a function of spoken or sung input and task.Journal of Music Therapy,19, 194–209.Google Scholar
  23. Kilgour, A. R., Jakobson, L. S., &Cuddy, L. L. (2000). Music training and rate of presentation as mediators of text and song recall.Memory & Cognition,28, 700–710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lerdahl, F., &Jackendoff, R. (1983).A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  25. McElhinney, M., &Annett, J. M. (1996). Pattern of efficacy of a musical mnemonic on recall of familiar words over several presentations.Perceptual & Motor Skills,82, 395–400.Google Scholar
  26. Medina, S. L. (1993). The effect of music on second language vocabulary acquisition.National Network for Early Language Learning,6, 1–8.Google Scholar
  27. Merker, B. (2004).Vocal learning for song: Key constraint on the path to language. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  28. Morrongiello, B. A., &Roes, C. L. (1990). Children’s memory for new songs: Integration or independent storage of words and tunes?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,50, 25–38.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L., &Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur Internet: Lexique.L’Année Psychologique,101, 447–462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Palmer, C., &Kelly, M. H. (1992). Linguistic prosody and musical meter in song.Journal of Memory & Language,31, 525–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Peretz, I. (1996). Can we lose memories for music? The case of music agnosia in a nonmusician.Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences,8, 481–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., Hébert, S., &Macoir, J. (2004). Singing in the brain: Insights from cognitive neuropsychology.Music Perception,21, 373–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Peretz, I., &Hyde, K. L. (2003). What is specific to music processing? Insights from congenital amusia.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,7, 362–367.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Peretz, I., Radeau, M., &Arguin, M. (2004). Two-way interactions between music and language: Evidence from priming recognition of tune and lyrics in familiar songs.Memory & Cognition,32, 142–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Racette, A., Bard, C., &Peretz, I. (2006). Making non-fluent aphasics speak: Sing along!Brain,129, 2571–2584.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Rainey, D. W., &Larsen, J. D. (2002). The effects of familiar melodies on initial learning and long-term memory for unconnected text.Music Perception,20, 173–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rey, A. (1964).L’examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  38. Rubin, D. C. (1995).Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of counting-out rhymes, ballads, and epics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Samson, S., &Zatorre, R. J. (1991). Recognition memory for text and melody of songs after unilateral temporal lobe lesion: Evidence for dual encoding.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 793–804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Scotto Di Carlo, N., &Germain, A. (1985). A perceptual study of the influence of pitch on the intelligibility of sung vowels.Phonetica,42, 188–197.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G., &Repp, B. H. (1984). Integration of melody and text in memory for songs.Cognition,16, 285–303.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Serafine, M. L., Davidson, J., Crowder, R. G., &Repp, B. H. (1986). On the nature of melody—text integration in memory for songs.Journal of Memory & Language,25, 123–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sloboda, J. A., &Parker, D. H. H. (1985). Immediate recall of melodies. In P. Howell, I. Cross, & R. West (Eds.),Musical structure and cognition (pp. 143–168). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  44. Thompson, W. F., &Russo, F. A. (2004). The attribution of emotion and meaning to song lyrics.Polskie Forum Psychologiczne,9, 51–62.Google Scholar
  45. Wallace, W. T. (1994). Memory for music: Effect of melody on recall of text.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,20, 1471–1485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wolfe, D. E., &Hom, C. (1993). Use of melodies as structural prompts for learning and retention of sequential verbal information by preschool students.Journal of Music Therapy,30, 100–118.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Amélie Racette
    • 1
  • Isabelle Peretz
    • 1
  1. 1.Département de psychologieUniversité de MontréalMontréalCanada

Personalised recommendations