Agnetta, B., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use. Animal Cognition, 3, 107–112.
Article
Google Scholar
Albuquerque, N., Guo, K., Wilkinson, A., Savalli, C., Otta, E., & Mills, D. (2016). Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biology Letters, 12, 20150883. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0883
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Amici, F., Cacchione, T., & Bueno-Guerra, N. (2017). Understanding of object properties by sloth bears, Melursus ursinus. Animal Behaviour, 134, 217–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.028
Article
Google Scholar
Arden, R., Bensky, M. K., & Adams, M. J. (2016). A review of cognitive abilities in dogs, 1911 through 2016: More individual differences, please! Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 307–312. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416667718
Article
Google Scholar
Attard, M. R. G., Chamoli, U., Ferrara, T. L., Rogers, T. L., & Wroe, S. (2011). Skull mechanics and implications for feeding behaviour in a large marsupial carnivore guild: The thylacine, Tasmanian devil and spotted-tailed quoll. Journal of Zoology, 285, 292–300.
Article
Google Scholar
Bailey, T. N. (2005). The African leopard: Ecology and behavior of a solitary felid (2nd ed.). Caldwell, NJ: Blackburn Press.
Google Scholar
Bailey, I., Myatt, J. P., & Wilson, A. M. (2013). Group hunting within the Carnivora: Physiological, cognitive and environmental influences on strategy and cooperation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 67, 1–17.
Article
Google Scholar
Baldaccini, N. E., Giunchi, D., Mongini, E., & Ragionieri, L. (2000). Foraging flights of wild rock doves (Columba l. livia): A spatio-temporal analysis. Italian Journal of Zoology, 67, 371–377.
Article
Google Scholar
Ban, S. D., Boesch, C., & Janmaat, K. R. L. (2014). Tai chimpanzees anticipate revisiting high-valued fruit trees from further distances. Animal Cognition, 17, 1353–1364.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Banszegi, O., Jacinto, E., Urrutia, A., Szenczi, P., & Hudson, R. (2017). Can but don’t: Olfactory discrimination between own and alien offspring in the domestic cat. Animal Cognition, 20, 795–804. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1100-z
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Baru, A. V., & Shmigidina, G. N. (1976). Role of auditory-cortex in discrimination of synthesized vowels by dogs. Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatelnosti Imeni I P Pavlova, 26, 1165–1173.
Google Scholar
Bastian, J (1967). The transmission of arbitrary environmental information between bottlenose dolphins. In R. G. Busnel (Eds.), Animal sonar: Biology and bionics, pp. 803–873. Jouy-en-Josas: Laboratorie de Physiologie Acoustique.
Bednarz, J. C. (1988). Cooperative hunting in Harris’ hawks (Parabuteo unicinctus). Science, 239, 1525–1527.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. (1977). Canis latrans. Mammalian Species, 79, 1–9.
Article
Google Scholar
Bensky, M. K., Gosling, S. D., & Sinn, D. L. (2013). The world from a dog’s point of view: A review and synthesis of dog cognition research. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 45, 209–406.
Article
Google Scholar
Benson-Amram, S., Dantzer, B., Stricker, G., Swanson, E. M., & Holekamp, K. E. (2016). Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 2532–2537. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1505913113
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Benson-Amram, S., Heinen, V. K., Gessner, A., Weldele, M. L., & Holekamp, K. E. (2014). Limited social learning of a novel technical problem by spotted hyenas. Behavioural Processes, 109, 111–120.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Benson-Amram, S., & Holekamp, K. E. (2012). Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 4087–4095.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Benson, J. F., & Patterson, B. R. (2015). Spatial overlap, proximity, and habitat use of individual wolves within the same packs. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 39, 31–40.
Article
Google Scholar
Bentley-Condit, V. K., & Smith, E. O. (2010). Animal tool use: Current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour, 147, 185–221; A1–A32.
Article
Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Parrish, A. E., Futch, S. E., Evans, T. A., & Perdue, B. M. (2015). Looking ahead? Computerized maze task performance by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129, 160–173.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Biondi, L. M., Bo, M. S., & Vassallo, A. I. (2008). Experimental assessment of problem solving by Milvago chimango (Aves: Falconiformes). Journal of Ethology, 26(1), 113–118.
Article
Google Scholar
Bitterman, M. E. (1965). Phyletic differences in learning. American Psychologist, 20(6), 396–410. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022328
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bizley, J. K., Walker, K. M. M., King, A. J., & Schnupp, J. W. H. (2013). Spectral timbre perception in ferrets: Discrimination of artificial vowels under different listening conditions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 133, 365–376.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Blasco, R., Finlayson, C., Rosell, J., Marco, A. S., Finlayson, S., Finlayson, G., … Vidal, J. R. (2014). The earliest pigeon fanciers. Scientific Reports, 4, 5971.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Boakes, R. A., & Gaertner, I. (1977). Development of a simple form of communication. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 561–575. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640747708400632
Article
Google Scholar
Boitani, L., & Ciucci, P. (1995). Comparative social ecology of feral dogs and wolves. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 7(1), 49–72. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927014.1995.9522969
Article
Google Scholar
Borrego, N., & Dowling, B. (2016). Lions (Panthera leo) solve, learn, and remember a novel resource acquisition problem. Animal Cognition, 19(5), 1019–1025. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-1009-y
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Borrego, N., & Gaines, M. (2016). Social carnivores outperform asocial carnivores on an innovative problem. Animal Behaviour, 114, 21–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.01.013
Article
Google Scholar
Brakke, K. E., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1995). The development of language skills in bonobo and chimpanzee: I. Comprehension. Language and Communication, 15, 121–148.
Article
Google Scholar
Bravo, M., Blake, R., & Morrison, S. (1988). Cats see subjective contours. Vision Research, 28(8), 861–865.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., & Belger, J. (2018). A ball is not a Kong: Odor representation and search behavior in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different education. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132(2), 189–199. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000115
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., Bos, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task. Animal Cognition, 16(2), 273–285. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0571-1
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., & Call, J. (2011). The magic cup: Great apes and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) individuate objects according to their properties. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 353–361.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., & Call, J. (2015). Apes produce tools for future use. American Journal of Primatology, 77, 254–263.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(2), 145–154. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.145
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 10(4), 439–s. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0088-1
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., Schönefeld, K., & Call, J. (2013). When do dogs help humans?. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 148(1), 138-149.
Article
Google Scholar
Briefer, E. F., Haque, S., Baciadonna, L., & McElligott, A. G. (2014). Goats excel at learning and remembering a highly novel cognitive task. Frontiers in Zoology, 11(1), 20.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Broekhuis, F., Thuo, D., & Hayward, M. W. (2018). Feeding ecology of cheetahs in the Maasai Mara, Kenya and the potential for intra- and interspecific competition. Journal of Zoology, 304, 65–72.
Article
Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Schubiger, M. N., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2017). The evolution of general intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e195. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X16000959
Brush, F. R., Brush, E. S., & Solomon, R. L. (1955). Traumatic avoidance learning: The effects of CS-US interval with a delayed-conditioning procedure. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 48, 285–293.
Article
Google Scholar
Butler, J. R. A., Du Toit, J. T., & Bingham, J. (2004). Free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) as predators and prey in rural Zimbabwe: Threats of competition and disease to large wild carnivores. Biological Conservation, 115, 369–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3207(03)00152-6
Article
Google Scholar
Careau, V., Giroux, J. F., & Berteaux, D. (2007). Cache and carry: Hoarding behavior of arctic fox. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62, 87-96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-007-0441-z
Article
Google Scholar
Catala, A., Mang, B., Wallis, L., & Huber, L. (2017). Dogs demonstrate perspective taking based on geometrical gaze following in a Guesser–Knower task. Animal Cognition, 20(4), 581–589. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1082-x
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Cattet, J., & Etienne, A. S. (2004). Blindfolded dogs relocate a target through path integration. Animal Behaviour, 68, 203–212.
Article
Google Scholar
Cavoto, K. K., & Cook, R. G. (2001). Cognitive precedence for local information in hierarchical stimulus processing by pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 3–16.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Chapuis, N. (1975). Inverse pathway learning in the dog. Cahiers de Psychologie, 18, 219–228.
Google Scholar
Clarke, R. S., Heron, W., Fetherstonhaugh, M. L., Forgays, D. G., & Hebb, D. O. (1951). Individual differences in dogs: Preliminary report on the effects of early experience. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 5, 150–156.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Clay, Z., Palagi, E., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2018). Ethological approaches to empathy in primates. In K. Meyza & E. Knapska (Eds.), Neuronal correlates of empathy (pp. 53–66). London, UK: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-805397-3.00005-X
Chapter
Google Scholar
Clayton, N. S., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature, 395, 272–274.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, J. (1995). Origins of the dog: Domestication and early history. In J. Serpell, (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour, and interactions with people. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Colbert-White, E. N., McCord, E. M., Sharpe, D. I., & Fragaszy, D. M. (2013). String-pulling behaviour in a Harris’s hawk Parabuteo unicinctus. Ibis, 155, 611–615.
Article
Google Scholar
Collier-Baker, E., Davis, J. M., & Suddendorf, T. (2004). Do dogs (Canis familiaris) understand invisible displacement? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 421–433.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Cook, R. G., & Fowler, C. (2014). “Insight” in pigeons: Absence of means-end processing in displacement tests. Animal Cognition, 17, 207–220.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Coppinger, R., & Coppinger, L. (2001). Dogs: A startling new understanding of canine origin, behavior and evolution. New York, NY: Scribner.
Google Scholar
Coppinger, R., & Coppinger, L. (2016). What is a dog? Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Coutellier, L. (2006). Are dogs able to recognize their handler's voice? A preliminary study. Anthrozoös, 19, 278–284.
Article
Google Scholar
Custance, D., & Mayer, J. (2012). Empathic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to distress in humans: An exploratory study. Animal Cognition, 15, 851–859.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
D’Aniello, B., Alterisio, A., Scandurra, A., Petremolo, E., Iommelli, M. R., & Aria, M. (2017). What’s the point? Golden and Labrador retrievers living in kennels do not understand human pointing gestures. Animal Cognition, 20, 777–787. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1098-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dinets, V. (2015). Apparent coordination and collaboration in cooperatively hunting crocodilians. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 27, 244–250.
Article
Google Scholar
Dinets, V. (2017). Coordinated hunting by Cuban boas. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 4, 24–29.
Article
Google Scholar
Domjan, M., & Galef, B. G. (1983). Biological constraints on instrumental and classical-conditioning: Retrospect and prospect. Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 151–161. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199643
Article
Google Scholar
Doré, F. Y., & Dumas, C. (1987). Psychology of animal cognition: Piagetian studies. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 219–233.
Article
Google Scholar
Doré, F. Y., & Goulet, S. (1992). Cognition in carnivorous mammals: Cats and dogs. Psychologie Française, 37, 65–72.
Google Scholar
Drea, C. M., & Carter, A. N. (2009). Cooperative problem solving in a social carnivore. Animal Behaviour, 78, 967–977.
Article
Google Scholar
Driscoll, C. A., Macdonald, D. W., & O'Brien, S. J. (2009). From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 9971–9978 Supplem. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901586106
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Dumas, C. (1998). Figurative and spatial information and search behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris). Behavioural Processes, 42(2/3), 101–106.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dumont, B., & Petit, M. (1998). Spatial memory of sheep at pasture. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 60, 43–53.
Article
Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., & Shultz, S. (2007). Understanding primate brain evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 649–658. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2001
Article
Google Scholar
Duranton, C., Rodel, H. G., Bedossa, T., & Belkhir, S. (2015). Inverse sex effects on performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in a repeated problem-solving task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129, 84–87.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Edwards, G. R., Newman, J. A., Parsons, A. J., & Krebs, J. R. (1996). The use of spatial memory by grazing animals to locate food patches in spatially heterogeneous environments: An example with sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 50, 147–160.
Article
Google Scholar
Epstein, R., Kirshnit, C. E., Lanza, R. P., & Rubin, L. C. (1984). Insight in the pigeon: Antecedents and determinants of an intelligent performance. Nature, 308, 61–62.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fabrigoule, C. (1974). Experimentation on spatial learning in the dog: Learning of progressive exclusion. Cahiers de Psychologie, 17, 91–110.
Google Scholar
Fabrigoule, C. (1976). Cue selection with the dog in a double ‘T’ maze has multiple solutions. Année Psychologique, 23, 59–381.
Google Scholar
Fabrigoule, C., & Sagave, I. (1992). Reorganization of cues and path organisation in dogs. Behavioural Processes, 28(1/2), 65–79.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fagot, J., & Tomonaga, M. (1999). Global and local processing in humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Use of a visual search task with compound stimuli. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 3–12.
Article
Google Scholar
Fields, P. E. (1936). Studies in concept formation: III. A note on the retention of visual figure discriminations. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 21, 131–136.
Article
Google Scholar
Figueirido, B., & Janis, C. M. (2011). The predatory behaviour of the thylacine: Tasmanian tiger or marsupial wolf? Biology Letters, 7, 937.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Fischel, W. (1933). Das Verhalten von Hunden bei doppelter Zielsetzung und doppelter Handlungsmoeglichkeit [The behavior of dogs with double goal setting and two possible actions]. Zeitschrift fuer Vergleichende Physiologie, 19, 170–182.
Google Scholar
Fiset, S. (2009). Evidence for averaging of distance from landmarks in the domestic dog. Behavioural Processes, 81, 429–438.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fiset, S., Beaulieu, C., & Landry, F (2003). Duration of dogs’ (Canis familiaris) working memory in search for disappearing objects. Animal Cognition, 6, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-002-0157-4
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fiset, S., Beaulieu, C., LeBlanc, V., & Dube, L. (2007). Spatial memory of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) for hidden objects in a detour task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 497–508.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fiset, S., & Doré, F. Y. (1996). Spatial encoding in domestic cats (Felis catus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 420–437.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fiset, S., Gagnon, S., & Beaulieu, C. (2000). Spatial encoding of hidden objects in dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 315–324.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fiset, S., & LeBlanc, V (2007). Invisible displacement understanding in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): The role of visual cues in search behavior. Animal Cognition, 10, 211–224. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0060-5
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fiset, S., & Plourde, V (2013). Object permanence in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and gray wolves (Canis lupus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127, 115–127.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
Frank, H. (1980). Evolution of canine information processing under conditions of natural and artificial selection. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 53, 389–399.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Frank, H. (2011). Wolves, dogs, rearing and reinforcement: Complex interactions underlying species differences in training and problem-solving performance. Behavior Genetics, 41, 830–839. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-011-9454-5
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1985). Comparative manipulation-test performance in ten-week-old wolves (Canis lupus) and Alaskan malamutes (Canis familiaris): A Piagetian interpretation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 266–274.
Article
Google Scholar
Fugazza, C., & Miklósi, Á. (2014). Deferred imitation and declarative memory in domestic dogs. Animal Cognition, 17(2), 237–247.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fugazza, C., & Miklósi, Á. (2015). Social learning in dog training: The effectiveness of the do as I do method compared to shaping/clicker training. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 171, 146–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.033
Article
Google Scholar
Fugazza, C., Pogány, Á., & Miklósi, Á. (2016). Do as I . . . Did! Long-term memory of imitative actions in dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 19(2), 263–269.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fujita, K., Morisaki, A., Takaoka, A., Maeda, T., & Hori, Y. (2012). Incidental memory in dogs (Canis familiaris): Adaptive behavioral solution at an unexpected memory test. Animal Cognition, 15, 1055–1063.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fuhrer, N., & Gygax, L. (2017). From minutes to days the ability of sows (Sus scrofa) to estimate time intervals. Behavioural Processes, 142, 146–155. https://doi.org/10.10164/j.beproc.2007.07.006
Gácsi, M., Kara, E., Belényi, B., Topál, J., & Miklósi, Á. (2009). The effect of development and individual differences in pointing comprehension of dogs. Animal Cognition, 12, 471–479.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gallagher, T. (2008). Falcon fever. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–87.
Article
Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G., Povinelli, D. J., Suarez, S. D., Anderson, J. R., Lethmate, J., & Menzel E. W. (1995). Further reflections on self-recognition in primates. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1525–1532.
Article
Google Scholar
Gagnon, S., & Doré, F. Y (1992). Search behavior in various breeds of adult dogs (Canis familiaris): Object permanence and olfactory cues. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 58–68.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gagnon, S., & Doré, F. Y (1994). Cross-sectional study of object permanence in domestic puppies (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 220–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.108.3.220
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gatti, R. C. (2016). Self-consciousness: Beyond the looking-glass and what dogs found there. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 28, 232–240.
Google Scholar
Gaulin, S. J. C., & Fitzgerald, R. W. (1986). Sex-differences in spatial ability: An evolutionary hypothesis and test. American Naturalist, 127, 74-88.
Article
Google Scholar
Gazda, S. K., Connor, R. C., Edgar, R. K., & Cox, F. (2005). A division of labour with role specialization in group-hunting bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off Cedar Key, Florida. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272, 135–140.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gehrt, S. D., Riley, S. P., & Cypher, B. L. (Eds.). (2010). Urban carnivores: Ecology, conflict, and conservation. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Kiraly, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755–755. https://doi.org/10.1038/415755a
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gieling, E. T., Nordquist, R. E., & van der Staay, F. J. (2011). Assessing learning and memory in pigs. Animal Cognition, 14, 151–173.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Gipson, P. S. (1983). Evaluation and control implications of behavior of feral dogs in interior Alaska. In D. E. Kaukeinen (Ed.), Vertebrate pest control and management materials: Fourth symposium. West Conshohocken, PA: ASTM International. https://doi.org/10.1520/STP817-EB
Chapter
Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1964). Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature, 201, 1264–1266.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Goulet, S., Doré, F. Y., & Rousseau, R. (1994). Object permanence and working memory in cats (Felis catus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20(4), 347–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.20.4.347
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Green, P. A., Van Valkenburgh, B., Pang, B., Bird, D., Rowe, T., & Curtis, A. (2012). Respiratory and olfactory turbinal size in canid and arctoid carnivorans. Journal of Anatomy, 221, 609–621.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Greenberg, J. R., Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Chimpanzee helping in collaborative and noncollaborative contexts. Animal Behaviour, 80, 873–880.
Article
Google Scholar
Gruber, H. E., Girgus, J. S., & Banuazizi, A (1971). The development of object permanence in the cat. Developmental Psychology, 4, 9-–15.
Article
Google Scholar
Hall, K. R. L., & Schaller, G. B. (1964). Tool-using behavior of the California sea otter. Journal of Mammalogy, 45, 287–298.
Article
Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition, 101, 495–514.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 439–444.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hart, V., Novákova, P., Malkemper, E. P., Begall, S., Hanzal, V., Ježek, M., … Burda, H. (2013). Dogs are sensitive to small variations of the Earth’s magnetic field. Frontiers in Zoology, 10, 80.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Hayes, K. J., & Hayes, C. (1952). Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 45, 450–459.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hayward, M. W. (2006). Prey preferences of the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta). Journal of Zoology, 270, 606–614.
Article
Google Scholar
Hayward, M. W., Lyngdoh, S., & Habib, B. (2014). Diet and prey preferences of dholes. (Cuon alpinus): Dietary competition within Asia’s apex predator guild. Journal of Zoology, 294, 255–266.
Article
Google Scholar
Hayward, M. W., O’Brien, J., Hofmeyr, M., & Kerley, G. I. H. (2006). Prey preferences of the African wild dog Lycaon pictus (Canidae: Carnivora): Ecological requirements for conservation. Journal of Mammalogy, 87, 1122–1131.
Article
Google Scholar
Head, E., Mehta, R., Hartley, J., Kameka, M., Cummings, B. J., Cotman, C. W.,… Milgram, N. W. (1995). Spatial learning and memory as a function of age in the dog. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 851–858. https://doi.org/10.1037//0735-7044.109.5.851
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Heberlein, M. T., Manser, M. B., & Turner, D. C. (2017). Deceptive-like behaviour in dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 20(3), 511–520.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hector, D. P. (1986). Cooperative hunting and its relationship to foraging success and prey size in an avian predator. Ethology, 73, 247–257.
Article
Google Scholar
Heishman, M., Conant, M., & Pasnak, R. (1995). Human analog tests of the 6th stage of object permanence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80, 1059–1068. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1995.80.3c.1059
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Held, S., Mendl, M., Devereux, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2000). Social tactics of pigs in a competitive foraging task: The ‘informed forager’ paradigm. Animal Behaviour, 59, 569–576.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Held, S., Mendl, M., Devereux, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2001). Behaviour of domestic pigs in a visual perspective taking task. Behaviour, 138, 1337–1354.
Article
Google Scholar
Hepper, P. G. (1988). The discrimination of human odour by the dog. Perception, 17, 549–554.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Herman, L. M. (2002). Vocal, social, and self-imitation by bottlenosed dolphins. In K. Dautenhahn & C. L. Nehaniv. (Eds.), Imitation in animals and artifacts. Complex adaptive systems (pp. 63–108). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Hewitson, L., Dumont, B., & Gordon, I. J. (2005). Response of foraging sheep to variability in the spatial distribution of resources. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1069–1076.
Article
Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Reflections on self-recognition in primates. Animal Behaviour, 47, 909–919.
Article
Google Scholar
Hienz, R. D., Aleszczyk, C. M., & May, B. J. (1996). Vowel discrimination in cats: Acquisition effects of stimulus level, and performance in noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, 3656–3668.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hiestand, L. (2011). A comparison of problem-solving and spatial orientation in the wolf (Canis lupus) and dog (Canis familiaris). Behavior Genetics, 41, 840–857.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hille, P., Dehnhardt, G., & Mauck, B. (2006). An analysis of visual oddity concept learning in a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Learning & Behavior, 34, 144–153.
Article
Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (Eds.). (1973). Constraints on learning. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Hobson, E. S. (1963). Feeding behavior in three species of sharks. Pacific Science, 17, 171–194.
Google Scholar
Holekamp, K. E., Boydston, E. E., Szykman, M., Graham, I., Nutt, K. J., Birch, S., . . . Singh, M. (1999). Vocal recognition in the spotted hyaena and its possible implications regarding the evolution of intelligence. Animal Behaviour, 58, 383–395. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1999.1157
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Holekamp, K. E., Sakai, S. T., & Lundrigan, B. L. (2007). The spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta) as a model system for study of the evolution of intelligence. Journal of Mammalogy, 88, 545–554.
Article
Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., & Washburn, D. A. (2002). Matching visual stimuli on the basis of global and local features by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Animal Cognition, 5, 27–31.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Hopper, L. M., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2015). The importance of witnessed agency in chimpanzee social learning of tool use. Behavioural Processes, 112, 120–129.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Huber, A., Barber, A. L. A., Faragó, T., Müller, C. A., & Huber, L. (2017). Investigating emotional contagion in dogs (Canis familiaris) to emotional sounds of humans and conspecifics. Animal Cognition, 20, 703–715. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1092-8
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Huber, L., Popovová, N., Riener, S., Salobir, K., & Cimarelli, G. (2018). Would dogs copy irrelevant actions from their human caregiver? Learning and Behavior. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-018-0336-z
Article
Google Scholar
Huber, L., Range, F., & Virányi, Z. (2014). Dog imitation and its possible origins. In A. Horowitz (Ed.), Domestic dog cognition and behavior (pp. 79–100). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Huber, L., Range, F., Voelkl, B., Szucsich, A., Virányi, Z., & Miklósi, Á. (2009). The evolution of imitation: What do the capacities of non-human animals tell us about the mechanisms of imitation? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 2299–2309. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0060
Article
Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. G. Bateson & R. A Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Hyvarinen, H., Palviainen, A., Strandberg, U., & Holopainen, I. J. (2009). Aquatic environment and differentiation of vibrissae: Comparison of sinus hair systems of ringed seal, otter and pole cat. Brain Behavior and Evolution, 74, 268–279.
Article
Google Scholar
Itakura, S., Das, L., & Farshid, A. (2017). Gaze processing in non-human primates. In R. Flom, K. Lee, & D. Muir (Eds.) Gaze-following: Its development and significance. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Jaakkola, K. (2014). Do animals understand invisible displacement? A critical review. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128, 225–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035675
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Jaakkola, K., Guarino, E., Rodriguez, M., Erb, L., & Trone, M. (2010). What do dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) understand about hidden objects? Animal Cognition, 13, 103–120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0250-z
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Jacobs, I. F., & Osvath, M. (2015). The string-pulling paradigm in comparative psychology. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129(2), 89–120.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Ban, S. D., & Boesch, C. (2013). Chimpanzees use long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across seasons. Animal Behaviour, 86, 1183-1205.
Article
Google Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Polansky, L., Ban, S. D., & Boesch, C. (2014). Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(46), 16343–16348.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M., Barrera, F. J., Ireland, C., & Woodside, B. (1978). Signal-centered action patterns of dogs in appetitive classical conditioning. Learning and Motivation, 9, 272–296.
Article
Google Scholar
Johnson, C. M., Sullivan, J., Buck, C. L., Trexel, J., & Scarpuzzi, M (2015). Visible and invisible displacement with dynamic visual occlusion in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops spp). Animal Cognition, 18, 179–193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0788-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Johnston, A. M., Holden, P. C., & Santos, L. R. (2017). Exploring the evolutionary origins of overimitation: A comparison across domesticated and non-domesticated canids. Developmental Science, 20(4).
Article
Google Scholar
Johnston, R. F. (1992). Evolution in the rock dove: skeletal morphology. Auk, 109, 530-542.
Google Scholar
Kalmus, H. (1955). The discrimination by the nose of the dog of individual human odours and in particular the odours of twins. British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 3, 1–25.
Article
Google Scholar
Kamil, A. C. (1998). On the proper definition of cognitive ethology. In R. P. Balda, I. M. Pepperberg, & A. C. Kamil (Eds.), Animal cognition in nature: The convergence of psychology and biology in laboratory and field (pp. 1–28). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human’s perspective. Behaviour, 146, 979–998.
Article
Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Fischer, J. (2004). Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for “fast mapping”. Science, 304, 1682–1683.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.010
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Fischer, J., & Call, J. (2008). Prospective object search in dogs: Mixed evidence for knowledge of what and where. Animal Cognition, 11, 367–371.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., & Nitzschner, M. (2013). Do dogs get the point? A review of dog–human communication ability. Learning and Motivation, 44, 294–302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2013.05.001
Article
Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 69(1), 11–18.
Article
Google Scholar
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Differing views: Can chimpanzees do Level 2 perspective-taking? Animal Cognition, 19, 555–564. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0956-7
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kendrick, K. M., Atkins, K., Hinton, M. R., Broad, K. D., Fabre-Nys, C., & Keverne, B. (1995). Facial and vocal discrimination in sheep. Animal Behaviour, 49, 1665–1676.
Article
Google Scholar
Kendrick, K. M., da Costa, A. P., Leigh, A. E., Hinton, M. R., & Peirce, J. W. (2001). Sheep don’t forget a face. Nature, 414, 165–166.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
King, S. L., Allen, S. J., Connor, R. C., & Jaakkola, K. (2016). Cooperation or dolphin ‘tug-of-war’? Comment on Kuczaj et al. and Eskelinen et al. Animal Cognition, 19, 1227–1229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-1026-x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kirchhofer, K. C., Zimmermann, F., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing. PLOS ONE, 7. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030913
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. London, UK: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Google Scholar
Kouwenberg, A.-L., Walsh, C. J., Morgan, B. E., & Martin, G. M. (2009). Episodic-like memory in crossbred Yucatan minipigs (Sus scrofa). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 117, 165–172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.01.005
Article
Google Scholar
Krauze-Gryz, D., & Gryz, J. (2014). Free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in central Poland: Density, penetration range and diet composition. Polish Journal of Ecology, 62, 183–193. https://doi.org/10.3161/104.062.0101
Article
Google Scholar
Krueger, K., & Flauger, B. (2011). Olfactory recognition of individual competitors by means of faeces in horse (Equus caballus). Animal Cognition, 14(2), 245-257.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Krützen, M., Mann, J., Heithaus, M. R., Connor, R. C., Bejder, L., & Sherwin, W. B. (2005). Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 8939–8943.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., Winship, K. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2015). Can bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) cooperate when solving a novel task? Animal Cognition, 18, 543–550.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Laca, E. A. (1998). Spatial memory and food searching mechanisms of cattle. Journal of Range Management, 51, 370–378.
Article
Google Scholar
Laughlin, K., & Mendl, M. (2004). Costs of acquiring and forgetting information affect spatial memory and its susceptibility to interference. Animal Behaviour, 68, 97–103.
Article
Google Scholar
Leach, H. M. (2003). Human domestication reconsidered. Current Anthropology, 44, 349–368.
Article
Google Scholar
Lemasson, A., Mikus, M. A., Blois-Heulin, C., & Lode, T. (2013). Social partner discrimination based on sounds and scents in Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinereus). Naturwissenschaften, 100, 275–279.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lindsey, P. A., du Toit, J. T., & Mills, M. G. L. (2004). Area and prey requirements of African wild dogs under varying habitat conditions: Implications for reintroductions. South African Journal of Wildlife Research, 34, 77–86.
Google Scholar
Li, X., Glaser, D., Li, W. H., Johnson, W. E., O’Brien, S. J., Beauchamp, G. K., & Brand, J. G. (2009). Analyses of sweet receptor gene (Tas1r2) and preference for sweet stimuli in species of carnivora. Journal of Heredity, 100, S90–S100.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Li, X., Li, W. H., Wang, H., Bayley, D. L., Cao, J., Reed, D. R., . . . Brand, J. G. (2006). Cats lack a sweet taste receptor. Journal of Nutrition, 136, 1932S–1934S.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Macdonald, D. W. (1976). Food caching by red foxes and some other carnivores. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 42, 170–185. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1976.tb00963.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Macdonald D. W., & Carr, G. M. (1995). Variation in dog society: Between resource dispersion and social flux. In J. Serpell (Eds.), The domestic dog (pp. 199–216). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Herrmann, E., Suchindran, S., & Hare, B. (2017). Individual differences in cooperative communicative skills are more similar between dogs and humans than chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 126, 41–51.
Article
Google Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1974). The psychology of animal learning. London, UK: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Macphail, E. M. (1987). The comparative psychology of intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 645–656. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00054984
Article
Google Scholar
Macpherson, K., & Roberts, W. A. (2006). Do dogs (Canis familiaris) seek help in an emergency? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 113–119.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Macpherson, K., & Roberts, W. A. (2010). Spatial memory in dogs (Canis familiaris) on a radial maze. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124, 47–56.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Maes, E., DeFilippo, G., Inkster, A. B., Lea, S. E. G., DeHouwer, J., D’Hooge, R.,… Wills, A. J. (2015). Feature- versus rule-based generalization in rats, pigeons and humans. Animal Cognition, 18, 1267–1284.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Maginnity, M. E., & Grace, R. C. (2014). Visual perspective taking by dogs (Canis familiaris) in a Guesser–Knower task: Evidence for a canine theory of mind? Animal Cognition, 17, 1375–1392.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Malassis, R., & Delfour, F. (2015). Sea lions’ (Zalophus californianus) use of human pointing gestures as referential cues. Learning & Behavior, 43, 101–112. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-014-0165-7
Article
Google Scholar
Malavasi, R., & Huber, L. (2016). Evidence of heterospecific referential communication from domestic horses (Equus caballus) to humans. Animal Cognition, 19(5), 899–909.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Marshall-Pescini, S., Rao, A., Virányi, Z., & Range, F. (2017). The role of domestication and experience in ‘looking back’ towards humans in an unsolvable task. Scientific Reports, 7, 46636.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Marshall-Pescini, S., Colombo, E., Passalacqua, C., Merola, I., & Prato-Previde, E. (2013). Gaze alternation in dogs and toddlers in an unsolvable task: evidence of an audience effect. Animal Cognition, 16(6), 933-943.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Marshall-Pescini, S., Schwarz, J. F. L., Kostelnik, I., Virányi, Z., & Range, F. (2017). Importance of a species' socioecology: Wolves outperform dogs in a conspecific cooperation task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114, 11793-11798. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1709027114
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Marshall-Pescini, S., Valsecchi, P., Petak, I., Accorsi, P. A., & Prato-Previde, E. (2008). Does training make you smarter? The effects of training on dogs' performance (Canis familiaris) in a problem solving task. Behavioural Processes, 78, 449-454.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Marten, K., & Psarakos, S. (1995). Using self-view television to distinguish between self-examination and social behavior in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 205–225.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Martin-Malivel, J., & Okada, K. (2007). Human and chimpanzee face recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Role of exposure and impact on categorical perception. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 1145–1155.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mauck, B., & Dehnhardt, G. (2005). Identity concept formation during visual multiple-choice matching in a harbor seal (Phoca vitulina). Learning & Behavior, 33, 428–436.
Article
Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1942). Systematics and the origin of species from the viewpoint of a zoologist. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
Google Scholar
McAdam, D. (1964). Effects of positional relations between subject CS + US on shuttle-box avoidance learning in cats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 58, 302–304.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
McGill, T.E. (1980). Amputation of vibrissae in show dogs. International Journal for the Study of Animal Problems, 1(6), 359–361.
Google Scholar
McLean, A. N. (2004). Short-term spatial memory in the domestic horse. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 85(1), 93–105.
Article
Google Scholar
McVey, J. M., Cobb, D. T., Powell, R. A., Stoskopf, M. K., Bobling, J. H., Waits, L. P., & Moorman, C. E. (2013). Diets of sympatric red wolves and coyotes in northeastern North Carolina. Journal of Mammalogy, 94, 1141–1148.
Article
Google Scholar
Mech, L. D., & Boitani, L. (2003). Wolves: Behavior, ecology, and conservation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Book
Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: Tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour, 72, 275–286.
Article
Google Scholar
Mellgren, R. L., & Roper, T. J. (1986). Spatial learning and discrimination of food patches in the European badger (Meles L.). Animal Behaviour, 34, 1129–1134.
Article
Google Scholar
Mendes, N., & Call, J. (2014). Chimpanzees form long-term memories for food locations after limited exposure. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 485–495.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mersmann, D., Tomasello, M., Call, J., Kaminski, J., & Taborsky, M. (2011). Simple mechanisms can explain social learning in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Ethology, 117, 675–690.
Article
Google Scholar
Met, A., Miklósi, Á., & Lakatos, G. (2014). Gaze-following behind barriers in domestic dogs. Animal Cognition, 17, 1401–1405. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0754-z
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Michels, K. M., Johnson, J. I., & Pustek, J. J. (1961). Solution of patterned-strings problems by raccoons. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54, 439–441.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Miklósi, Á. (2014). Dog cognition, behaviour and evolution (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Virányi, Z., & Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big difference: Wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Current Biology, 13, 763–766.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Miklósi, Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (1998). Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Animal Cognition, 1, 113–121.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Miklósi, Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2000). Intentional behaviour in dog–human communication: An experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog. Animal Cognition, 3, 159–166.
Article
Google Scholar
Miklósi, Á., & Soproni, K. (2006). A comparative analysis of animals' understanding of the human pointing gesture. Animal Cognition, 9, 81–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-005-0008-1
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Miller, H. C., Gipson, C. D., Vaughan, A., Rayburn-Reeves, R., & Zentall, T. R. (2009). Object permanence in dogs: Invisible displacement in a rotation task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 150–155. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.1.150
Article
Google Scholar
Miller, H. C., Rayburn-Reeves, R., & Zentall, T. R. (2009a). Imitation and emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure. Behavioural Processes, 80, 109–114.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Miller, H. C., Rayburn-Reeves, R., & Zentall, T. R. (2009b). What do dogs know about hidden objects? Behavioural Processes, 81, 439–446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2009.03.018
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Mitchell, R. W., & Thompson, N. S. (1993). Familiarity and the rarity of deception: Two theories and their relevance to play between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 291–300.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Morin, A. (2011). Self-recognition, theory-of-mind, and self-awareness: What side are you on? Laterality, 16, 367–383. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576501003702648
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Müller, C. A., Mayer, C., Dorrenberg, S., Huber, L., & Range, F. (2011). Female but not male dogs respond to a size constancy violation. Biology Letters, 7, 689–691. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.0287
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Müller, C. A., Riemer, S., Range, F., & Huber, L. (2014a). Dogs’ use of the solidity principle: revisited. Animal Cognition, 17(3), 821–825.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Müller, C. A., Riemer, S., Range, F., & Huber, L. (2014b). The use of a displacement device negatively affects the performance of dogs (Canis familiaris) in visible object displacement tasks. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128, 240–250. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036032
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Müller, C. A., Riemer, S., Virányi, Z., Huber, L., & Range, F. (2014). Dogs learn to solve the support problem based on perceptual cues. Animal Cognition, 17, 1071–1080.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Müller, C. A., Riemer, S., Virányi, Z., Huber, L., & Range, F. (2016). Inhibitory control, but not prolonged object-related experience appears to affect physical problem-solving performance of pet dogs. PLOS One, 11. e147753
Google Scholar
Muro, C., Escobedo, R., Spector, L., & Coppinger, R. P. (2011). Wolf-pack (Canis lupus) hunting strategies emerge from simple rules in computational simulations. Behavioural Processes, 88(3), 192–197.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Nakajima, S., Sato, M. (1993). Removal of an obstacle: Problem-solving behavior in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 131–145.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Nawroth, C., Baciadonna, L., & McElligott, A. G. (2016). Goats learn socially from humans in a spatial problem-solving task. Animal Behaviour, 121, 123–129.
Article
Google Scholar
Nawroth, C., Brett, J. M., & McElligott, A. G. (2016). Goats display audience-dependent human-directed gazing behaviour in a problem-solving task. Biology Letters, 12(7), 20160283.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Nawroth, C., Ebersbach, M., & Von Borell, E. (2013). A note on pigs’ knowledge of hidden objects. Archiv für Tierzucht, 56, 861–872. https://doi.org/10.7482/0003-9438-56-086
Article
Google Scholar
Nawroth, C., Ebersbach, M., & Von Borell, E. (2014). Juvenile domestic pigs (Sus scrofa domestica) use human-given cues in an object choice task. Animal Cognition, 17, 701–713. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0702-3
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Nawroth, C., Von Borell, E., & Langbein, J. (2015). Object permanence in the dwarf goat (Capra aegagrus hircus): Perseveration errors and the tracking of complex movements of hidden objects. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 167, 20–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.03.010
Article
Google Scholar
Nguyen, D. T., Lee, K., Choi, H., Choi, M. K., Le, M. T., Song, N., … Kim, T. H. (2012). The complete swine olfactory subgenome: Expansion of the olfactory gene repertoire in the pig genome. BioMedCentral Genomics, 13(1), 584– 596.
Article
Google Scholar
Oden, D. L., Premack, D., & Thompson, R. K. R. (1988). Spontaneous transfer of matching by infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 140–145.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Osthaus, B., Lea, S. E. G., & Slater, A. M. (2003). Training influences problem-solving abilities in dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of British Society of Animal Science, 103, 103.
Google Scholar
Osthaus, B., Lea, S. E. G., & Slater, A. M. (2005). Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a string-pulling task. Animal Cognition, 8, 37–47.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Osthaus, B., Marlow, D., & Ducat, P. (2010). Minding the gap: spatial perseveration error in dogs. Animal Cognition, 13(6), 881–885.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Osthaus, B., Slater, A. M., & Lea, S. E. G. (2003). Can dogs defy gravity? A comparison with the human infant and a non-human primate. Developmental Science, 6, 489–497.
Article
Google Scholar
Ostojić, L., & Clayton, N. S. (2014). Behavioural coordination of dogs in a cooperative problem-solving task with a conspecific and a human partner. Animal Cognition, 17, 445–459.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ostrander, E. A., Wayne, R. K., Freedman, A. H., & Davis, B. W. (2017). Demographic history, selection and functional diversity of the canine genome. Nature Reviews Genetics, 18, 705–720. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2017.67
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Osvath, M., & Karvonen, E. (2012). Spontaneous innovation for future deception in a male chimpanzee. PLOS ONE, 7, e36782.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Osvath, M., & Osvath, H. (2008). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition, 11, 661–674.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pack, A. A., & Herman, L. M. (2004). Bottlenosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referent of both static and dynamic human gazing and pointing in an object-choice task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 160–171. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.118.2.160
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pack, A. A., Herman, L. M., Hoffmann-Kuhnt, M., & Branstetter, B. K. (2002). The object behind the echo: Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) perceive object shape globally through echolocation. Behavioural Processes, 58, 1–26.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Parr, L. A., Winslow, J. T., Hopkins, W. D., & DeWaal, F. B. M. (2000). Recognizing facial cues: Individual discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 47–60.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Parra, D., Méndez, S., Cañón, J., & Dunner, S. (2008). Genetic differentiation in pointing dog breeds inferred from microsatellites and mitochondrial DNA sequence. Animal Genetics, 39, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2052.2007.01658.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pattison, K. F., Laude, J. R., & Zentall, T. R. (2013). The case of the magic bones: Dogs’ memory of the physical properties of objects. Learning and Motivation, 44, 252–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2013.04.003
Article
Google Scholar
Pattison, K. F., Miller, H. C., Rayburn-Reeves, R., & Zentall, T. (2010). The case of the disappearing bone: Dogs’ understanding of the physical properties of objects. Behavioural Processes, 85, 278–282.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Google Scholar
Perdue, B. M., Snyder, R. J., Zhihe, Z., Marr, J., & Maple, T. L. (2011). Sex differences in spatial ability: A test of the range size hypothesis in the order Carnivora. Biology Letters, 7, 380–383.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Perri, A. (2016). A wolf in dog’s clothing: Initial dog domestication and Pleistocene wolf variation. Journal of Archaeological Science, 68, 1–4.
Article
Google Scholar
Persson, M. E., Roth, L. S. V., Johnsson, M., Wright, D., & Jensen, P. (2015). Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 14(4), 337–344.
Article
Google Scholar
Petter, M., Musolino, E., Roberts, W. A., & Cole, M. (2009). Can dogs (Canis familiaris) detect human deception? Behavioural Processes, 82, 109–118.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pettit, M. (2010). The problem of raccoon intelligence in behaviourist America. British Journal For the History of Science, 43, 391–421.
Article
Google Scholar
Pietrzykowska, B., & Soltysik, S. (1975a). A failure to train the “same–different” differentiation of photic stimuli in dogs. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 35, 27–38.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pietrzykowska, B., & Soltysik, S. (1975b). Transfer of the “same–different” differentiation task in dogs. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 35, 39–50.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pilley, J. W., & Reid, A. K (2011). Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behavioural Processes, 86, 184–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.11.007
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pinc, L., Bartoš, L., Reslová, A., & Kotrba, R. (2011). Dogs discriminate identical twins. PLOS ONE, 6, e20704.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Piotti, P., & Kaminski, J. (2016). Do dogs provide information helpfully?. PLOS ONE, 11(8), e0159797.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Pitcher, B. J., Harcourt, R. G., & Charrier, I. (2010). The memory remains: long-term vocal recognition in Australian sea lions. Animal Cognition, 13, 771-776.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pitteri, E., Mongillo, P., Carnier, P., & Marinelli, L. (2014). Hierarchical stimulus processing by dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 17, 869–877.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Gurobi, K., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2001). Social learning in dogs: The effect of a human demonstrator on the performance of dogs in a detour task. Animal Behaviour, 62, 1109–1117. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1866
Article
Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). Factors influencing young chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) recognition of attention. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 336–345.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., Nelson, K. E., & Boysen, S. T. (1992). Comprehension of role reversal in chimpanzees: Evidence of empathy. Animal Behaviour, 43, 633–640.
Article
Google Scholar
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.
Article
Google Scholar
Price, E. O. (1999). Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 65(3), 245–271.
Article
Google Scholar
Proops, L., McComb, K., & Reby, D. (2009). Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 947–951.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Racca, A., Amadei, E., Ligout, S., Guo, K., Meints, K., & Mills, D. (2010). Discrimination of human and dog faces and inversion responses in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 13, 525–533.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Range, F., Moslinger, H., & Virányi, Z. (2012). Domestication has not affected the understanding of means-end connections in dogs. Animal Cognition, 15, 597–607.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Range, F., & Virányi, Z. (2011). Development of gaze following abilities in wolves (Canis lupus). PLOS ONE, 6. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0016888
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Range, F., & Virányi, Z. (2014). Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: the “canine cooperation hypothesis”. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1582.
Ratcliffe, V. F., & Reby, D. (2014). Orienting asymmetries in dogs’ responses to different communicatory components of human speech. Current Biology, 24, 2908–2912.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 5937–5942.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Riedel, J., Schumann, K., Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The early ontogeny of human-dog communication. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1003–1014.
Article
Google Scholar
Rizzari, J. R., & Lönnstedt, O. M. (2014). Cooperative hunting and gregarious behaviour in the zebra lionfish, Dendrochirus zebra. Marine Biodiversity, 44, 467–468. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12526-014-0215-6
Article
Google Scholar
Robbins, W. D., & Renaud, P. (2016). Foraging mode of the grey reef shark, Carcharhinus amblyrhynchos, under two different scenarios. Coral Reefs, 35, 253–260.
Article
Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1886). Animal intelligence (4th ed). London, UK: Kegan Paul, Trench.
Google Scholar
Romero, T., Ito, M., Saito, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2014). Social modulation of contagious yawning in wolves. PLOS ONE, 9. e105963.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Romero, T., Konno, A., & Hasegawa, T. (2013). Familiarity bias and physiological responses in contagious yawning by dogs support link to empathy. PLOS ONE, 8, e71365.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Rooijakkers, E. F., Kaminski, J., & Call, J. (2009). Comparing dogs and great apes in their ability to visually track object transpositions. Animal Cognition, 12, 789–796. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0238-8
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Rooney, S., & Sargan, D. (2008). Pedigree dog breeding in the UK: A major welfare concern?. RSPCA. Retrieved from https://www.rspca.org.uk/adviceandwelfare/pets/dogs/health/pedigreedogs/report
Rorvang, M. V., Ahrendt, L. P., & Christensen, J. W (2015). Horses fail to use social learning when solving spatial detour tasks. Animal Cognition, 18, 847–854. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0852-6
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Saito, A., & Shinozuka, K. (2013). Vocal recognition of owners by domestic cats (Felis catus). Animal Cognition, 16, 685–690.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sato, A., Kanazawa, S., & Fujita, K. (1997). Perception of object unity in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Japanese Psychological Research, 39, 191–199.
Article
Google Scholar
Schakner, Z. A., Petelle, M. B., Tennis, M. J., Van der Leeuw, B. K., Stansell, R. T., & Blumstein, D. T. (2017). Social associations between California sea lions influence the use of a novel foraging ground. Royal Society Open Science, 4, 160820. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160820
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Schmelz, M., Duguid, S., Bohn, M., & Volter, C. J. (2017). Cooperative problem solving in giant otters (Pteronura brasiliensis) and Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinerea). Animal Cognition, 20, 1107–1114.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Schmidberger, G. (1932). Uber die Bedeutung der Schnurrhaare bei Katzen [On the significance of the vibrissae in cats]. Zeitschrift fur vergleichende Physiologie, 17, 387–407.
Google Scholar
Scholtyssek, C., Kelber, A., Hanke, F. D., & Dehnhardt, G. (2013). A harbor seal can transfer the same/different concept to new stimulus dimensions. Animal Cognition, 16, 915–925.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sèbe, F., Duboscq, J., Aubin, T., Ligout, S., & Poindron, P. (2010). Early vocal recognition of mother by lambs: Contribution of low- and high-frequency vocalizations. Animal Behaviour, 79, 1055–1066.
Article
Google Scholar
Seguinot, V., Cattet, J., & Benhamou, S. (1998). Path integration in dogs. Animal Behaviour, 55, 787–797.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sekuler, A. B., Lee, J. A. J., & Shettleworth, S. J. (1996). Pigeons do not complete partly occluded figures. Perception, 25, 1109-1120.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406–418.
Article
Google Scholar
Seward, J. P., & Humphrey, G. L. (1967). Avoidance learning as a function of pretraining in cat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 338–341.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shapiro, A. D., Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J. B. (2003). A gray seal’s (Halichoerus grypus) responses to experimenter-given pointing and directional cues. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 355–362. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.117.4.355
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sharpe, L. L., Hill, A., & Cherry, M. I. (2013). Individual recognition in a wild cooperative mammal using contact calls. Animal Behaviour, 86, 893–900.
Article
Google Scholar
Shepherd, W. T. (1915). Tests on adaptive intelligence in dogs and cats, as compared with adaptive intelligence in Rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Psychology, 26, 211–216.
Article
Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, evolution and behavior (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Silva, K., Bessa, J., & de Sousa, L. (2012). Auditory contagious yawning in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): First evidence for social modulation. Animal Cognition, 15, 721–724.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Singer, R., & Henderson, E. (2015). Object permanence in marine mammals using the violation of expectation procedure. Behavioural Processes, 112, 108–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.08.025
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sklepkovych, B. O., & Montevecchi, W. A. (1996). Food availability and food hoarding behaviour by red and arctic foxes. Arctic, 49, 228–234.
Article
Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., & Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 17228–17233.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Smith, B. P., Appleby, R. G., & Litchfield, C. A. (2012). Spontaneous tool-use: An observation of a dingo (Canis dingo) using a table to access an out-of-reach food reward. Behavioural Processes, 89, 219–224.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Soley, F. G., & Alvarado-Díaz, I. (2011). Prospective thinking in a mustelid? Eira barbara (Carnivora) cache unripe fruits to consume them once ripened. Naturwissenschaften, 98(8), 693–698.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Solomon, R. L., & Wynne, L. C. (1953). Traumatic avoidance learning: acquisition in normal dogs. Psychological Monographs, 67(19).
Article
Google Scholar
Somppi, S., Tornqvist, H., Kujala, M. V., Hanninen, L., Krause, C. M., & Vainio, O. (2016). Dogs evaluate threatening facial expressions by their biological validity: Evidence from gazing patterns. PLOS ONE, 11, e143047.
Article
Google Scholar
Stander, P. E. (1992). Cooperative hunting in lions: The role of the individual. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29, 445–454.
Article
Google Scholar
Stanford, C. B., Wallis, J., Matama, H., & Goodall, J. (1994). Patterns of predation by chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe National Park, 1982–1991. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 94, 213–228.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167.
Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., Corballis, M. C., & Collier-Baker, E. (2009). How great is great ape foresight? Animal Cognition, 12, 751–754.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Surbeck, M., & Hohmann, G. (2008). Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park. Current Biology, 18(19), R906–R907.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Szenczi, P., Bánszegi, O., Urrutia, A., Faragó, T., & Hudson, R. (2016). Mother-offspring recognition in the domestic cat: Kittens recognize their own mother’s call. Developmental Psychobiology, 58, 568–577. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21402
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Szetei, V., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2003). When dogs seem to lose their nose: An investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 83, 141–152.
Article
Google Scholar
Tanida, H., & Nagano, Y. (1998). The ability of miniature pigs to discriminate between a stranger and their familiar handler. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 56(2), 149–159.
Article
Google Scholar
Teleki, G. (1973). The predatory behavior of wild chimpanzees. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press.
Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Book
Google Scholar
Thornton, A., & Samson, J. (2012). Innovative problem solving in wild meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 83, 1459-1468.
Article
Google Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1993). Behavior systems and reinforcement: An integrative approach. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 105–128. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1993.60-105
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1994). Behavior systems, associationism, and Pavlovian conditioning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 405–420. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210945
Article
Google Scholar
Topál, J., Byrne, R. W., Miklósi, A., & Csányi, V. (2006). Reproducing human actions and action sequences:“Do as I Do!” in a dog. Animal Cognition, 9(4), 355-367.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Topál, J., Gergely, G., Erdőhegyi, A., Csibra, G., & Miklósi, Á. (2009). Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants. Science, 325, 1269–1272. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1176960
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Triana, E., & Pasnak, R. (1981). Object permanence in cats and dogs. Animal Learning & Behavior, 9, 135–139.
Article
Google Scholar
Trinkel, M., Fleischmann, P. H., Steindorfer, A. F., & Kastberger, G. (2004). Spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) follow migratory prey: Seasonal expansion of a clan territory in Etosha, Namibia. Journal of Zoology, 264, 125–133.
Article
Google Scholar
Troje, N. F., Huber, L., Loidolt, M., Aust, U., & Fieder, M. (1999). Categorical learning in pigeons: The role of texture and shape in complex static stimuli. Vision Research, 39, 353–366.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In W. Donaldson & E. Tulving. (Eds.), Organization of memory. Oxford, UK: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychologist, 26, 1–12.
Article
Google Scholar
Tyack, P. L. (1997). Development and social functions of signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus. Bioacoustics, 8, 21–46.
Article
Google Scholar
Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2008). Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1767–1773.
Article
Google Scholar
Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010). The performance of stray dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a shelter on human-guided object-choice tasks. Animal Behaviour, 79, 717–725.
Article
Google Scholar
Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2011). Can your dog read your mind? Understanding the causes of canine perspective taking. Learning & Behavior, 39, 289–302.
Article
Google Scholar
Van Parijs, S. M., & Clark, C. W. (2006). Long-term mating tactics in an aquatic-mating pinniped, the bearded seal, Erignathus barbatus. Animal Behaviour, 72, 1269–1277.
Article
Google Scholar
Van Valkenburgh, B., Curtis, A., Samuels, J. X., Bird, D., Fulkerson, B., Meachen-Samuels, J., & Slater, G. J. (2011). Aquatic adaptations in the nose of carnivorans: Evidence from the turbinates. Journal of Anatomy, 218, 298–310.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Vanak, A. T., & Gompper, M. E. (2009). Dogs Canis familiaris as carnivores: their role and function in intraguild competition. Mammal Review, 39, 265–283. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2907.2009.00148.x
Article
Google Scholar
Vilà, C., & Wayne, R. K. (1999). Hybridization between wolves and dogs. Conservation Biology, 13, 195-198. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1523-1739.1999.97425.x
Article
Google Scholar
Virányi, Z., Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Belényi, B., Ujfalussy, D., & Miklósi, Á. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Animal Cognition, 11, 373–387.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Vonk, J., Jett, S. E., & Mosteller, K. W. (2012). Concept formation in American black bears, Ursus americanus. Animal Behaviour, 84, 953–964.
Article
Google Scholar
Vonk, J., & Johnson-Ulrich, Z. (2014). Social and nonsocial category discriminations in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and American black bears (Ursus americanus). Learning & Behavior, 42, 231–245.
Article
Google Scholar
Vonk, J., & Leete, J. A. (2017). Carnivore concepts: Categorization in carnivores “bears” further study. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 30, 1–20
Wackermannová, M., Pinc, L., & Jebavý, L. (2016). Olfactory sensitivity in mammalian species. Physiological Research, 65, 369–390.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wang, X., & Tedford, R. H. (2010). Dogs: Their fossil relatives and evolutionary history. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
Google Scholar
Waroff, A. J., Fanucchi, L., Robbins, C. T., & Nelson, O. L. (2017). Tool use, problem-solving, and the display of stereotypic behaviors in the brown bear (Ursus arctos). Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research, 17, 62–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.11.003
Article
Google Scholar
Washburn, S. L., & Lancaster, J. (1968). The evolution of hunting. In R. B. Lee & I. De Vore (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 293–303). Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Google Scholar
Watson, J. S., Gergely, G., Csányi, V., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., & Sarkozi, Z. (2001). Distinguishing logic from association in the solution of an invisible displacement task by children (Homo sapiens) and dogs (Canis familiaris): Using negation of disjunction. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 219–226. https://doi.org/10.1037//0735-7036.115.3.219
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Way, J. G., & Cabral, R. D. (2009). Effects of hierarchy rank on caching frequency in a captive coywolf (Eastern Coyote) Canis latrans x lycaon, pack. Canadian Field Naturalist, 123, 173–175. https://doi.org/10.22621/cfn.v123i2.699
Article
Google Scholar
Werhahn, G., Virányi, Z., Barrera, G., Sommese, A., & Range, F. (2016). Wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris) differ in following human gaze into distant space but respond similar to their packmates’ gaze. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130(3), 288–298.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wells, D. L., & Hepper, P. G. (2003). Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 84, 297–305.
Article
Google Scholar
West, B., & Zhou, B. X. (1988). Did chickens go North? New evidence for domestication. Journal of Archaeological Science, 15, 515–533.
Article
Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape?. Animal Learning & Behavior, 32(1), 36–52.
Article
Google Scholar
Whitt, E., Douglas, M., Osthaus, B., & Hocking, I. (2009). Domestic cats (Felis catus) do not show causal understanding in a string-pulling task. Animal Cognition, 12, 739–743.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., & Fitch, W. T. (2014). The "Domestication Syndrome" in mammals: A unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics, 197, 795–808. https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.114.165423
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wills, A. J., Lea, S. E. G., Leaver, L. A., Osthaus, B., Ryan, C. M. E., Suret, M., … Millar, L. (2009). A comparative analysis of the categorization of multidimensional stimuli: I. Unidimensional classification does not necessarily imply analytic processing; evidence from pigeons (Columba livia), squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) and humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123, 391–405.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Windholz, G. (1999). Protopopov’s ideas on habit formation and their relation to the Pavlovian theory of higher nervous activity. American Journal of Psychology, 112, 437–448.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Woodruff, G., & Premack, D. (1979). Intentional communication in the chimpanzee: Development of deception. Cognition, 7, 333–362.
Article
Google Scholar
Wozencraft, W. C. (2005). Order Carnivora. In D. E. Wilson & D. M. Reeder (Eds.), Mammal species of the world: A taxonomic and geographic reference (3rd ed., pp. 532–628). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Wrangham, R. W. (1979). On the evolution of ape social systems. Social Science Information, 18, 335–368. https://doi.org/10.1177/053901847901800301
Article
Google Scholar
Wright, A. A., Cook, R. G., Rivera, J. J., Sands, S. F., & Delius, J. D. (1988). Concept learning by pigeons: Matching-to-sample with trial-unique video picture stimuli. Animal Learning and Behavior, 16, 436–444.
Article
Google Scholar
Wright, H. F., Wilkinson, A., Croxton, R. S., Graham, D. K., Harding, R. C., Hodkinson, H. L., … Zulch, H. E. (2017). Animals can assign novel odours to a known category. Scientific Reports, 7. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-09454-0
Wyrwicka, W. (1994). Jerzy Konorski (1903–1973) on the 20th anniversary of his death. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 18, 449–453.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Xitco, M. J., Gory, J. D., & Kuczaj, S. A. (2001). Spontaneous pointing by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Animal Cognition, 4(2), 115–123.
Article
Google Scholar
Yosef, R., & Yosef, N. (2010). Cooperative hunting in brown-necked raven (Corvus rufficollis) on Egyptian Mastigure (Uromastyx aegyptius). Journal of Ethology, 28, 385–388. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-009-0191-7
Article
Google Scholar
Zaine, I., Domeniconi, C., & Wynne, C. D. L. (2015). The ontogeny of human point following in dogs: When younger dogs outperform older. Behavioural Processes, 119, 76–85.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Zamisch, V., & Vonk, J. (2012). Spatial memory in captive American black bears (Ursus americanus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 372–387.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Zentall, T. R., Clement, T. S., Bhatt, R. S., & Allen, J. (2001). Episodic-like memory in pigeons. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 685–690.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Zentall, T. R., & Pattison, K. F. (2016). Now you see it, now you don’t: Object permanence in dogs. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 357–362. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416664861
Article
Google Scholar