Advertisement

Central European Journal of Biology

, Volume 7, Issue 4, pp 596–602 | Cite as

The effect of rapid and depot testosterone and estradiol on spatial performance in water maze

  • Julius HodosyEmail author
  • Daniela Ostatnikova
  • Vladimír Riljak
  • Jaromir Myslivecek
  • Peter Celec
Research Article
  • 93 Downloads

Abstract

Men and women differ in some cognitive functions including spatial abilities. These differences seem to be affected by sex steroids, but the results are controversial. The aim of this work is to describe the effects of rapid or depot testosterone and estradiol on spatial memory in rats. Thirty-two adult male Wistar rats were divided into 6 groups. Five groups were gonadectomized, and one group was left as control. Castrated groups received sterile oil, testosterone isobutyras, testosterone propionate, estradiol dipropionate or estradiol benzoate. We evaluated spatial performance (escape latency, overall improvement, and time in the quadrant after platform removal) of the rats in a spatial water maze. Animals receiving exogenous sex steroids showed higher plasma concentrations of the particular hormones. Experimental groups improved during the acquisition spatial trials in the water maze. No significant differences between the groups during probe trial were found. In overall improvement, the testosterone depot and estradiol depot groups showed less improvement in comparison to the control groups (P<0.05). No differences in respect to administered hormones were found in corresponding receptor gene expression in hippocampus. In conclusion, exogenous testosterone affects spatial memory of adult castrated males.

Keywords

Water maze Spatial performance Testosterone Estradiol Hippocampus Rats 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. [1]
    Burkitt J., Widman D., Saucier D.M., Evidence for the influence of testosterone in the performance of spatial navigation in a virtual water maze in women but not in men, Horm Behav, 2007, 51, 649–654PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. [2]
    Rugarn O., Hammar M., Theodorsson A., Theodorsson E., Stenfors C., Sex differences in neuropeptide distribution in the rat brain, Peptides, 1999, 20, 81–86PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. [3]
    Mcewen B.S., When Is Stimulation Too Much of a Good Thing, Neurobiology of Aging, 1991, 12, 346–348PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. [4]
    Berenbaum S.A., Korman Bryk K., Duck S.C., Resnick S.M., Psychological adjustment in children and adults with congenital adrenal hyperplasia, J Pediatr, 2004, 144, 741–746PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. [5]
    Hier D.B., Crowley W.F., Jr., Spatial ability in androgen-deficient men, N Engl J Med, 1982, 306, 1202–1205PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. [6]
    Williams C.L., Barnett A.M., Meck W.H., Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation in spatial memory, Behav Neurosci, 1990, 104, 84–97PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. [7]
    Fitch R.H., Denenberg V.H., A role for ovarian hormones in sexual differentiation of the brain, Behav Brain Sci, 1998, 21, 311–327PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. [8]
    Squire L.R., Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans, Psychol Rev, 1992, 99, 195–231PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. [9]
    Naghdi N., Nafisy N., Majlessi N., The effects of intrahippocampal testosterone and flutamide on spatial localization in the Morris water maze, Brain Res, 2001, 897, 44–51PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. [10]
    Pouliot W.A., Handa R.J., Beck S.G., Androgen modulates N-methyl-D-aspartate-mediated depolarization in CA1 hippocampal pyramidal cells, Synapse, 1996, 23, 10–19PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. [11]
    Galea L.A., Kavaliers M., Ossenkopp K.P., Sexually dimorphic spatial learning in meadow voles Microtus pennsylvanicus and deer mice Peromyscus maniculatus, J Exp Biol, 1996, 199, 195–200PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. [12]
    Gurzu C., Artenie V., Hritcu L., Ciobica A., Prenatal testosterone improves the spatial learning and memory by protein synthesis in different lobes of the brain in the male and female rat, Cent Eur J Biol, 2008, 3, 39–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. [13]
    Grimshaw G.M., Sitarenios G., Finegan J.A., Mental rotation at 7 years: relations with prenatal testosterone levels and spatial play experiences, Brain Cogn, 1995, 29, 85–100PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. [14]
    Ulubaev A., Lee D.M., Purandare N., Pendleton N., Wu F.C., Activational effects of sex hormones on cognition in men, Clin Endocrinol (Oxf), 2009, 71, 607–623CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. [15]
    Celec P., Ostatnikova D., Putz Z., Kudela M., The circalunar cycle of salivary testosterone and the visual-spatial performance, Bratisl Lek Listy, 2002, 103, 59–69PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. [16]
    Spencer J.L., Waters E.M., Romeo R.D., Wood G.E., Milner T.A., McEwen B.S., Uncovering the mechanisms of estrogen effects on hippocampal function, Front Neuroendocrinol, 2008, 29, 219–237PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. [17]
    Cherrier M.M., Matsumoto A.M., Amory J.K., Johnson M., Craft S., Peskind E.R., et al, Characterization of verbal and spatial memory changes from moderate to supraphysiological increases in serum testosterone in healthy older men, Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2007, 32, 72–79PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. [18]
    Osborne D.M., Edinger K., Frye C.A., Chronic administration of androgens with actions at estrogen receptor beta have anti-anxiety and cognitive-enhancing effects in male rats, Age (Dordr), 2009, 31, 191–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. [19]
    Sandstrom N.J., Kim J.H., Wasserman M.A., Testosterone modulates performance on a spatial working memory task in male rats, Horm Behav, 2006, 50, 18–26PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. [20]
    Spritzer M.D., Gill M., Weinberg A., Galea L.A., Castration differentially affects spatial working and reference memory in male rats, Arch Sex Behav, 2008, 37, 19–29PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. [21]
    Moradpour F., Naghdi N., Fathollahi Y., Anastrozole improved testosterone-induced impairment acquisition of spatial learning and memory in the hippocampal CA1 region in adult male rats, Behav Brain Res, 2006, 175, 223–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. [22]
    Hodosy J., Celec P., Ostatnikova D., Caganova M., Putz Z., Balaz P., et al, The effect of anastrozol on spatial memory in adult rats, Cent Eur J Biol, 2009, 4, 186–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. [23]
    Harooni H.E., Naghdi N., Sepehri H., Rohani A.H., Intra hippocampal injection of testosterone impaired acquisition, consolidation and retrieval of inhibitory avoidance learning and memory in adult male rats, Behav Brain Res, 2008, 188, 71–77PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. [24]
    Naghdi N., Oryan S., Etemadi R., The study of spatial memory in adult male rats with injection of testosterone enanthate and flutamide into the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala in Morris water maze, Brain Res, 2003, 972, 1–8PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. [25]
    Naghdi N., Majlessi N., Bozorgmehr T., The effect of intrahippocampal injection of testosterone enanthate (an androgen receptor agonist) and anisomycin (protein synthesis inhibitor) on spatial learning and memory in adult, male rats, Behav Brain Res, 2005, 156, 263–268PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Versita Warsaw and Springer-Verlag Wien 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Julius Hodosy
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Daniela Ostatnikova
    • 2
  • Vladimír Riljak
    • 3
  • Jaromir Myslivecek
    • 3
  • Peter Celec
    • 1
    • 4
    • 5
  1. 1.Institute of Molecular BiomedicineComenius University, Faculty of MedicineBratislavaSlovakia
  2. 2.Institute of PhysiologyComenius University, Faculty of MedicineBratislavaSlovakia
  3. 3.Charles UniversityPragueCzech Republic
  4. 4.Institute of PathophysiologyComenius University, Faculty of MedicineBratislavaSlovakia
  5. 5.Department of Molecular BiologyComenius UniversityBratislavaSlovakia

Personalised recommendations