Systematic analysis of feeding behaviors and their effects on feed efficiency in Pekin ducks
- 1.8k Downloads
- 2 Citations
Abstract
Background
Feeding behavior study is important for animal husbandry and production. However, few studies were conducted on the feeding behavior and their relationship with feeding efficiency in Pekin ducks. In order to investigate the feeding behavior and their relationship with feed efficiency and other economic traits in Pekin ducks, we selected 358 male Pekin ducks and recorded feeding information between 3 to 6 wk of age using automatic electronic feeders, and compared the feeding behavior under different residual feed intake (RFI) levels.
Results
We observed that total feed time, daily feed intake and feed intake per meal had strong positive correlations with feed efficiency traits; moreover, strong correlation between feed intake per meal and body weight was found (R=0.32, 0.36). Daily feeding rate meal and meal duration had weak correlations with feed efficiency (R=0.14~0.15). The phenotypic correlation of between-meal pauses, with feed efficiency was not observed. When daily changes were analyzed, high RFI ducks had the highest feed consumption over all times, and obvious differences in daily visits were found among different RFI level animals during the middle period; these differences were magnified with age, but there was no difference in daily meal number. Moreover, our data indicate that high RFI birds mainly take their meals at the edge of the population enclosure, where they are more susceptible to environmental interference.
Conclusions
Overall, this study suggests that the general feeding behaviors can be accurately measured using automatic electronic feeders and certain feeding behaviors in Pekin ducks are associated with improved feed efficiency.
Keywords
Economic traits Feeding behavior Feed efficiency Pekin duckAbbreviations
- BMP
Between-meal pauses
- BW
Body weight
- BWG
Body weight gain
- DFI
Daily feed intake
- DFR
Daily feeding rate
- FCR
Feed conversion rate
- FI
Feed intake
- MD
Meal duration
- MFI
Feed intake per meal
- RFI
Residual feed intake
- RFID
Radio Frequency Identification
- TFT
Total feeding time
Background
Feed efficiency is a major economic trait of domestic animals. For most poultry, feed efficiency has been selected in variety of ways, such as feed conversion ratio (FCR) and residual feed intake (RFI) [1, 2]. Improvements in feed efficiency have been achieved by selecting for RFI [3, 4], and some studies have shown that FCR has the genetic potential to improve feed efficiency in Pekin ducks and mule ducks [5, 6]. New developed automatic recording machine helps us to account large number of individuals at the same time in breeding. In practice, we found ducks needs adaptation time at the beginning of using feeding machine. Adaptive learning ability was observed to be different in Pekin ducks at beginning of using machine. So learning animal feeding behavior would be important for animal husbandry, productions system design and animal welfare [7, 8].
It is widely recognized that behavior is an important aspect of the physiological status of animals [9]. Feeding behavior may reflect animal meal habit, as a potential predictor of feed efficiency [10]. Moreover, feeding behavior can also be used as an indicator for reflecting animal health situation. Early studies that utilized behavioral data depended on human observation or photographic information acquired by camera equipment. With the development of electronic feeders, individual feeding information can be collected automatically and measured accurately [11]. Recent studies observed the meal intervals showed considerable variations in Pekin ducks [12]. In broilers, feeding behaviors were found to be related with feed efficiency in different selected lines [13]. However, the general feeding behavior pattern is not clear and whether these feeding behavior affect feed efficiency in Pekin ducks is also unknown.
In order to investigate the relationship between feeding behavior and feed efficiency in Pekin ducks, this study selected 358 male Pekin ducks and recorded feeding information between 3 to 6 wk of age using electronic feeders. Our results provide basic feeding behavior data in Pekin ducks and found feeding behavior are strongly correlated with feed efficiency in Pekin ducks.
Methods
Birds and housing
All ducks were reared in the same house with the same feeding and lighting program. Each house hold 100 feeding machines. Ten machines were grouped together as one pen with barrier for duck management. We randomly selected 3 batches of 3 wk. Male Pekin ducks obtained from the Beijing Golden Star Company (358 birds in total). Each of the 3 batches (batch one: n = 126; batch two: n = 119; batch three: n = 113) was measured from 3 wk of age until 6 wk of age. The intervals between batches was 8 d and measurement was same for each batch. Ducks in each batch were fed ad libitum for 25 d in a single pen. The pen area was 96 m2 and contained 10 automatic feeders and a water tank. Illumination was 10 lx for 24 h during the entire period. The pen design is illustrated in Additional file 1: Figure S1.
Feeding behavior data acquisition
We designed duck feeding machine which use RFID as sensor to record each duck. The design of duck feeding machine is similar with Bley and Bessei [12]. Feeding behaviors (entering time, exiting time, feed consumption) and live body were recorded for each feeding for each duck from 19 d to 42 d. Only one duck can enter the machine for each feeding event adjusting barrier according to duck body size. The machine has been extensively used in the duck breeding practices [14]. Each feeding machine was spaced at 0.5 m intervals, and the feeder ID was set from 1 to 10 in linear. The first machine was nearest the gate and the last one was against the barrier. Tiny electronic tags were permanently fastened to the upper neck of each duck until slaughtering, which couldn’t interfere with animal activity. Upon entering a feeder, each duck’s electronic label ID was recognized by the machine. The date and time at beginning and end of each visit was recorded. The central server collected the raw data from each feeder and calculated feed intake and meal duration. At the end of feeding, the ducks with very small body size, without measurement or unusual feeding behavior were removed before analysis of feeding behavior. Any visit that could not be assigned to a specific duck was removed from the data set before analysis. During the whole measuring period, 0.001% of missing record events were observed and these records were removed. Only the ducks with complete records were used for further analysis. Additionally, the records that individual weight were far outweighed other records on the same day were removed, which may be caused by interference from other ducks.
Traits measurement
Based on the distribution of RFI, three subsets were selected for later analysis. Ducks in the bottom 10% of RFI were defined as the low-RFI level, from 45% to 55% were defined as the middle-RFI level and the top 10% RFI individuals were selected as the high-RFI group. Each subgroup had 33 ducks, and these ducks were randomly chosen from the combined data.
Every visit was treated as a valid record if feed intake and meal duration were greater than zero. The intervals between visits were calculated to estimate the meal criterion (Additional file 2: Figure S2). The meal criterion was estimated using the method 1 developed by Howie et al. [15]. For each duck, visits were defined as part of a meal if the interval between visits was lower than the meal criterion. Feeding behavior traits were calculated daily and over the test period for each duck: feed intake, feeding rates, average intervals between meals, number of visits and number of meals. The distribution of records was also analyzed to investigate the feeding tendency of ducks.
Because feeding patterns of animals are affected by the environment and their growth needs, the data has been divided into three stages, based on previous analysis. The first three days were considered as a period of adaptation, the fourth day to the tenth day after the commencement of the study was defined as the pre-growth period, and the eleventh to twentieth day was classified as the growth period.
Data analysis
In formation, level j was the RFI levels, and the batch i was the batch effect. E ij was the residual of the model. Additionally interaction effect between batch and RFI level was not significant in the data.
Results
Descriptive statistics and meal interval estimation
Descriptive statistics of measured traits for Pekin ducks
Items | All (n = 358) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | S.D. | Min | Max | ||
General traits | BW 19, kg | 1.1 | 0.09 | 0.74 | 1.4 |
BW 42, kg | 3.8 | 0.2 | 3.1 | 4.4 | |
BWG, kg | 2.7 | 0.2 | 1.8 | 3.3 | |
Feed Intake, kg | 7.2 | 0.8 | 3.9 | 9.4 | |
RFI, kg | 0.0 | 0.6 | −3.9 | 2.1 | |
FCR | 2.7 | 0.3 | 1.3 | 3.4 | |
Feeding behavior traits | Total Feed Time, min | 320.6 | 72.9 | 204.9 | 616.6 |
Daily feed intake, kg/d | 0.324 | 0.037 | 0.234 | 0.669 | |
Meal Feed Intake, g | 33.8 | 22.8 | 1.0 | 350.0 | |
Visit duration, s | 76.6 | 20.0 | 31.0 | 149.3 | |
Meal duration, s | 159.2 | 162.4 | 79.9 | 316.8 | |
Average interval between meals, min | 161.8 | 123 | 91.0 | 309.5 | |
Daily Feeding Rate, g/min | 20.3 | 3.7 | 14.0 | 34.1 | |
Number of meals per day | 8.6 | 1.4 | 2 | 14.4 | |
Number of visits per day | 13.6 | 3.1 | 8.9 | 28.8 |
Phenotypic correlation
Phenotypic correlation between feeding and other traits
Total feed time | Daily feed intake | Daily feeding rate | Between-meal pause | Meal feed intake | Meal duration | BW 19 | BW 42 | BWG | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BW 19 | 0.08 | 0.33** | 0.10 | 0.14* | 0.36** | 0.15** | 1.00 | 0.44** | −0.07 |
BW 42 | 0.22** | 0.62** | 0.07 | −0.13* | 0.32** | 0.1 | 0.36** | 1.00 | 0.90** |
BWG | 0.20** | 0.44** | 0.03 | −0.19** | 0.11* | 0.04 | −0.07 | 0.9** | 1.00 |
RFI | 0.23** | 0.58** | 0.14* | −0.07 | 0.23** | 0.12* | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.00 |
FCR | 0.16** | 0.45** | 0.14* | 0.06 | 0.27** | 0.15** | 0.44** | −0.13* | −0.34** |
FI | 0.31** | 0.84** | 0.15* | −0.12* | 0.33** | 0.16** | 0.31** | 0.65** | 0.55** |
Comparison among different RFI level
Comparison of measured traits among different RFI levels
Items | Low RFI(n = 33) | Middle RFI(n = 33) | High RFI(n = 33) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | S.D. | Mean | S.D. | Mean | S.D. | ||
General Traits | BW 19, kg | 1.1 | 0.1 | 1.1 | 0.1 | 1.2 | 0.1 |
BW 42, kg | 3.9 | 0.3 | 3.8 | 0.2 | 3.9 | 0.2 | |
Feed intake, kg | 6.5c | 0.7 | 7.2b | 0.4 | 8.4a | 1.1 | |
RFI | −0.9c | 0.59 | −0.024b | 0.027 | 0.9a | 1.1 | |
FCR | 2.3b | 0.2 | 2.7a | 0.1 | 2.7a | 0.19 | |
Feeding behavior traits | Total feed time, min | 297.8B | 65.1 | 321.1AB | 61.0 | 346.1A | 98.5 |
Daily feed intake, kg/d | 0.297C | 0.024 | 0.324B | 0.025 | 0.372A | 0.061 | |
Visit feed intake, g | 23 | 5 | 23 | 5 | 25 | 5 | |
Meal feed intake, g | 33b | 7 | 37b | 6 | 40a | 8 | |
Visit duration, s | 76.1 | 20.7 | 75.4 | 21.3 | 77.5 | 22.3 | |
Meal duration, s | 147.5c | 38.8 | 171.8a | 40.4 | 170.1a | 41.7 | |
Daily interval between meals, min | 165 | 26 | 167 | 23 | 161 | 28 | |
Daily feeding rate, g/min | 19.6 | 3.0 | 20.3 | 4.3 | 21.7 | 4.0 | |
Number of meals per day | 8.5 | 3.3 | 8.4 | 3 | 8.7 | 3.2 | |
Number of visits per day | 12.9 | 5.9 | 13.8 | 5.8 | 14.5 | 6.6 |
Phenotypic correlation between feeding behavior and other traits in different RFI groups
Total Feed Time | Daily Feed Intake | Daily Feeding Rate | Between-Meal Pause | Meal Feed Intake | Meal Duration | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RFI Group | H | M | L | H | M | L | H | M | L | H | M | L | H | M | L | H | M | L |
BW 19 | 0 | −0.11 | 0.31 | 0.19 | 0.52** | 0.33 | −0.03 | 0.24 | −0.12 | 0.14 | 0.18 | 0.43* | 0.14 | 0.41* | 0.48** | −0.01 | 0.13 | 0.01 |
BW 42 | 0.21 | −0.01 | 0.32 | 0.48** | 0.79** | 0.79** | −0.11 | 0.32 | 0.1 | 0.12 | −0.06 | 0.04 | 0.25 | 0.33 | 0.46** | 0.18 | 0.06 | 0.08 |
BWG | 0.22 | 0.05 | 0.23 | 0.41* | 0.65** | 0.73** | −0.10 | 0.24 | 0.16 | 0.03 | −0.18 | −0.12 | 0.20 | 0.15 | 0.32 | 0.25 | −0.01 | 0.08 |
FI | 0.41* | −0.02 | 0.55** | 0.77** | 0.8** | 0.54** | 0.16 | 0.31 | 0.03 | −0.36* | −0.03 | 0.02 | 0.18 | 0.36* | 0.26 | 0.5** | 0.08 | 0.12 |
RFI | 0.36* | 0.13 | 0.38* | 0.67** | 0.08 | −0.03 | 0.39* | −0.1 | −0.04 | −0.4* | 0.1 | −0.05 | 0.1 | 0.11 | −0.11 | 0.45** | 0.16 | 0.08 |
FCR | 0.32 | −0.09 | 0.39* | 0.35* | 0.09 | −0.1 | 0.30 | 0.04 | −0.13 | 0.10 | 0.29 | 0.15 | 0.08 | 0.3 | 0 | 0.41* | 0.15 | 0.05 |
Changes in feeding behavior over time
Evolution of feeding behavior traits during the feeding test period. X axis denotes the age (days), and Y axis denotes daily mean of each feeding behavior trait. Error bars are illustrated on the plots. The data from the last day was removed, since ducks are considered to be under stress during this fasting period. Ducks were 19 d of age at the beginning of the experiment. a Daily feed intake (b) Number of daily visits (c) Number of daily meals (d) Daily feeding rate (e) Daily meal duration (f) Between-meal pause
Feeding tendency
a The distribution of visits recorded by feeders. X axis denotes the feeder ID, and Y axis denotes the number of visits. Different colors represent three RFI levels of the ducks, respectively. Feeding machine is linearly layout from number one to ten. The first machine was closest to the gate, and the last one was far away from gate. All of invalid visits were removed. b Comparison of duck feeding tendencies in different RFI level groups. X axis represents the age of birds, and Y axis represents the distance between feeder and door. The box plot represents the range of feeding activities at different RFI levels. The data of the last day was removed, because at this fasting time the ducks were considered to be under stress. Variance analysis showed that the machine position that high RFI ducks tended to eat in was significantly lower (i.e. closer to the gate) than other ducks, over the entire time period (P < 0.01). The initial bird age was 19 d
Distribution of daily feeding time
The feeding visit distribution during the day. X axis denotes the time points (hr), Y axis denotes the number of visits. The results for three different periods are illustrated: 19–21 day-old; 22–28 day-old; 29–38 day-old; the initial bird age was 19 d. The number of each RFI group is 33
Discussion
Meal determination
Feeding behavior consists of different feeding activities, which are referred to feeder visits. Bley and Bessei [12] found that Pekin ducks exhibited an average of 29 visits per day at 4 wk of age, decreasing to fewer than 15 visits per day at 7 wk of age. Basso et al. [11] obtained a similar trend with 27 visits per day at 3 wk, rapidly decreasing to 15 visits, and then stabilizing. In this study, we found that the number of visits was gradually reduced from an average 25 visits per day at 3 wk of age to 14 visits at 4 wk, and then was stable at about 13 visits until the end of the study at 7 wk (Fig. 1b).
Our results are consistent with the two studies above. Actually a meal is not always equal to one visit, but rather contained a few visits and short relaxation pauses called within-meal pauses for animal relaxing or drinking; frequently, meals are separated by non-feeding intervals called between-meal pauses which are much longer than within-meal pause [11]. Therefore, a meal criterion is needed for grouping different feeding events into defined meals [15, 16]. Howie et al. [17] obtained a meal criterion of 1725 s for Pekin ducks by fitting a truncated log-normal model, and Basso et al. [11] found the meal criterion of 1790 s for Mule ducks using same method; however, Drouilhet et al. [18] estimated the meal criterion as 2208 s in their study in which the feeder entrance had a door. In our study, the log distribution of visit intervals is similar to the trends reported by Howie et al. [15] and Basso et al. [11] (Additional file 2: Figure S2); hence, we used Howie’s method for our data set and calculated a meal criterion of 1,132 s for Pekin ducks. This value is, lower than the meal criterion reported by others, which is likely due to the differences in feeder construction and in duck population. We found that meals consisted on average of 1.7 visits and that the number of meal per day had a similar trend in three RFI-level. Basso et al. [11] and Drouilhet et al. [18] found that the number of meals tended to decrease with age; however, between-meal pauses tended to increase. In boilers, Howie et al. [15] also found same trend. Our results showed that the number of meals tended to decrease with age (from 12 meal/d to 7 meal/d) after feeder adaption time and the between-meal pauses tended to increase (from 60 min/d to 2 h/d more). The results are in agreement with previous studies.
Feeding tendency at different RFI levels
Feeding tendency is possibly affected by environmental factors, especially under a wide range of farming conditions. These changes may be due to management mechanisms or system structures. For example, sward structure and herbage quality affected cattle’s grazing behavior [19]. Another study showed that the average time cattle spent lying increased with increasing number of concentrate feeders [20]. Funston et al. [21] found different group composition affected cattle feeding tendencies. In ducks, Drouilhet et al. [18] used a feeder with a door closing behind the animals, and reported that this affected the visit intervals. In our study, most ducks tended to eat at the inside feeders which are far away from the gate. However, the innermost feeder was not the best feeding position, since the feeding machine slightly near the center have the highest recorded number of visits (Fig. 2a). This suggests that ducks spontaneously move away from the gate where animals are susceptible to environmental interference. In contrast, the innermost machine is limited by proximity to the fence and this could greatly reduce activity space for ducks. Our results also show that high RFI ducks tend to eat near the gate, where high RFI ducks were more susceptible to outside interface (Fig. 2b), and this always occurs during the entire period. Some studies indicate that selection for feed efficiency might reduce behavioral reactivity [22], and evidence reveals that animals with better feed efficiency have lower fearfulness. Genetic research with mammals and poultry showed that low RFI animals were less active [23, 24]. Drouilhet, et al. [18] reported that selection for low RFI produces mule ducks that are slightly better adapted to human activity, in agreement with a previous study [25]. Compare to their results, our study suggests that high RFI individuals have lower adaptive ability to the new feeding environment. However, there is not enough evidence to determine the clear reason for this effect. Moreover, we found that feeding time points for adaptation during the first 3 d were different than at other time periods (Fig. 3). Ducks mainly were eating at noon and early afternoon during first 3 d while adapting to the feeders. After adaptation, ducks returned to middle of the night eating habits. These behaviors may be improved by feeder structure modification and selection for adaption response.
Feeding behavior at different RFI levels
The relationship between feeding behavior and feeding efficiency depends on population and species, and there are obvious differences in feeding behavior between mammals and poultry selected for feed efficiency. Studies with cattle reported that high RFI cattle consume more feed, but with similar feeding rates, compared to low RFI animals [26, 27]. For pigs, Young et al. [28] found that low RFI animals had shorter feeding times with a lower DFI compared to the high RFI pigs, and that the feeding rates of low RFI animals were significantly faster than the control group. A similar result was obtained in a hen study; Braastad and Katle [23] found that low RFI birds had shorter feeding times than high RFI birds. Drouilhet et al. [18] found that low RFI mule ducks had significantly lower DFI than high RFI ducks, but no differences were significant between high and low RFI lines for other feeding behavior traits. In this study, we found there was a significant phenotypic correlation between feed efficiency and feeding behaviors except for BMP (Table 2). It indicated that the feeding frequency has a limited impact on feed efficiency, total feeding time and daily feed intake are more critical. The Low RFI ducks spent less feeding time and had a lower daily feed intake than High RFI ducks. However these correlative relationships were not observed in different RFI-level sub-populations (Table 4). Therefore, selection for RFI may change the association between feeding behavior and other traits.
Our results were different with those of Drouilhet, et al. [18]; this discrepancy could be caused by feeder structure and selection of meal criterion. Additionally, when compared day by day, ducks in the three different RFI levels had a similar number of daily visits at 4 wk of age; however, after 10 d of the test period, low RFI ducks had a significantly lower visit number than high RFI ducks, and these differences were magnified with age (Fig. 1b). Over the entire test period, these changes were too microscopic to achieve statistical significance. Our results gave a possibility that selection for RFI might change the unknown mechanisms that underlie control of feeding behavior, but more genetic evidences were needed to support the hypothesis. This showed that selection for feeding behavior is a valuable approach for trait improvement.
Conclusions
In this study, we selected 358 male Pekin ducks and measured their feeding behavior, feed efficiency traits in order to analyze relationships between feeding behavior and feed efficiency. The results illustrate that the feeding tendency of Pekin ducks is obviously different at different RFI levels, and that high RFI ducks prefer the feeders closed to side barriers. The results also suggests that certain feeding behaviors have the potential to improve other economic traits with an improvement in feed efficiency. Further research will focus on genetic factors to gain insight into the relationship between feeding behavior and feed efficiency.
Notes
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Maxwell Hincke (University of Ottawa, Canada) for a careful review of the manuscript.
Funding
The work was supported by the National Scientific Supporting Projects of China (2015BAD03B06), National Waterfowl-industry Technology Research System (CARS-42) and the Program for Changjiang Scholars and Innovative Research Team in University (IRT_15R62).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Authors’ contributions
FZ carried out the experiment and drafted the manuscript. ZCH conceived the study, participated in its design and coordination, and helped draft the manuscript. FBL, JPH, FXY helped sample and experiment. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All experiments were performed according to regulations and guidelines established by the Animal Care and Use Committee of China Agricultural University (permit number: DK996). All protocols and procedures were approved by the Beijing Administration Committee of Laboratory Animals under the leadership of the Beijing Association for Science and Technology (permit number: SYXK 2007–0023). All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering during the study.
Consent for publication
Not applicable
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Supplementary material
References
- 1.Bezerra LR, Sarmento JL, Neto SG, de Paula NR, Oliveira RL. Do Rego WM. Residual feed intake: a nutritional tool for genetic improvement. Trop Anim Health Prod. 2013;45(8):1649–61.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 2.Koch RM, Gregory KE, Chambers D, Swiger LA. Efficiency of feed use in beef cattle. J Animal Sci. 1963;22(2):486–94.Google Scholar
- 3.Basso B, Bordas A, Dubos F, Morganx P, Marie-Etancelin C. Feed efficiency in the laying duck: appropriate measurements and genetic parameters. Poult Sci. 2012;91(5):1065–73.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 4.Kennedy BW, van der Werf JH, Meuwissen TH. Genetic and statistical properties of residual feed intake. J Anim Sci. 1993;71(12):3239–50.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 5.Pingel H. Influence of breeding and management on the efficiency of duck production. Lohmann information. 1999;22:7–13.Google Scholar
- 6.Larzul C, Guy G, Bernadet MD. Feed efficiency, growth and carcass traits in female mule ducks. Archiv Fur Geflugelkunde. 2004;68(6):265–8.Google Scholar
- 7.Howie JA, Tolkamp BJ, Avendano S, Kyriazakis I. The structure of feeding behavior in commercial broiler lines selected for different growth rates. Poult Sci. 2009;88(6):1143–50.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 8.Houpt KA. Animal behavior and animal welfare. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 1991;198(8):1355–60.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 9.Pennisi E. Genetics. A genomic view of animal behavior. Science. 2005;307(5706):30–2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 10.Schwartzkopf-Genswein KS, Atwood S, Mcallister TA. Relationships between bunk attendance, intake and performance of steers and heifers on varying feeding regimes. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;76(3):179–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 11.Basso B, Lague M, Guy G, Ricard E, Marie-Etancelin C. Detailed analysis of the individual feeding behavior of male and female mule ducks. J Anim Sci. 2014;92(4):1639–46.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 12.Bley TA, Bessei W. Recording of individual feed intake and feeding behavior of pekin ducks kept in groups. Poult Sci. 2008;87(2):215–21.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 13.Howie JA, Avendano S, Tolkamp BJ, Kyriazakis I. Genetic parameters of feeding behavior traits and their relationship with live performance traits in modern broiler lines. Poult Sci. 2011;90(6):1197–205.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 14.Bian B, Hao J, Yang F, Hu S, Hou Z. Study on feeding behavior and the relationship between residual feed intake and some economic traits in Peking ducks. China Poultry. 2014;36(16):8–11.Google Scholar
- 15.Howie JA, Tolkamp BJ, Avendaño S, Kyriazakis I. A novel flexible method to split feeding behaviour into bouts. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;116(116):101–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 16.Yeates MP, Tolkamp BJ, Allcroft DJ, Kyriazakis I. The use of mixed distribution models to determine bout criteria for analysis of animal behaviour. J Theor Biol. 2001;213(3):413–25.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 17.Howie JA, Tolkamp BJ, Bley T, Kyriazakis I. Short-term feeding behaviour has a similar structure in broilers, turkeys and ducks. Br Poult Sci. 2010;51(6):714–24.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 18.Drouilhet L, Monteville R, Molette C, Lague M, Cornuez A, Canario L, et al. Impact of selection for residual feed intake on production traits and behavior of mule ducks. Poult Sci. 2016;95(9):1999–2010.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- 19.Stejskalova M, Hejcmanova P, Pavlu V, Hejcman M. Grazing behavior and performance of beef cattle as a function of sward structure and herbage quality under rotational and continuous stocking on species-rich upland pasture. Anim Sci J. 2013;84(8):622–9.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 20.Gonzalez LA, Ferret A, Manteca X, Ruiz-de-la-Torre JL, Calsamiglia S, Devant M, et al. Effect of the number of concentrate feeding places per pen on performance, behavior, and welfare indicators of Friesian calves during the first month after arrival at the feedlot. J Anim Sci. 2008;86(2):419–31.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 21.Funston RN, Kress DD, Havstad KM, Doornbos DE. Grazing behavior of rangeland beef cattle differing in biological type. J Anim Sci. 1991;69(4):1435–42.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 22.Canario L, Mignon-Grasteau S, Dupont-Nivet M, Phocas F. Genetics of behavioural adaptation of livestock to farming conditions. Animal. 2013;7(3):357–77.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 23.Braastad BO, Katle J. Behavioural differences between laying hen populations selected for high and low efficiency of food utilisation. Br Poult Sci. 1989;30(3):533–44.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 24.Luiting P, Urff EM. Optimization of a model to estimate residual feed consumption in the laying hen. Livest Prod Sci. 1991;27(4):321–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 25.Arnaud I, Mignon-Grasteau S, Larzul C, Guy G, Faure JM, Guemene D. Behavioural and physiological fear responses in ducks: genetic cross effects. Animal. 2008;2(10):1518–25.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 26.Lancaster PA, Carstens GE, Ribeiro FR, Tedeschi LO, Jr CD. Characterization of feed efficiency traits and relationships with feeding behavior and ultrasound carcass traits in growing bulls. J Anim Sci. 2009;87(4):1528–39.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 27.Golden JW, Kerley MS, Kolath WH. The relationship of feeding behavior to residual feed intake in crossbred Angus steers fed traditional and no-roughage diets. J Anim Sci. 2008;86(1):180–6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- 28.Young JM, Cai W, Dekkers JC. Effect of selection for residual feed intake on feeding behavior and daily feed intake patterns in Yorkshire swine. J Anim Sci. 2011;89(3):639–47.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
Copyright information
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.