Introduction

Disruption of gamma rhythm affects learning-induced neuronal ensembles [1], and specific theta-gamma relationships underpin entorhinal-hippocampal communication and episodic memory [2]. Greater theta-gamma coupling predicts successful encoding [3]. Previous studies have shown alterations in hippocampal theta-gamma cross-frequency coupling prior to Aβ overproduction in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) transgenic mice [4]. Loss of gamma-band synchronization has also been reported in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD dementia [5, 6]. Evidence of AD-related alteration of neural oscillations has been translated into encouraging findings of potential benefit from brain stimulation, especially in the gamma range [7].

Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) entrains endogenous EEG oscillations in a specific frequency, which was suggested to have effects on cognitive improvements in aging population [8]. 40 Hz tACS has gained recent attention as an intervention for AD, and limited randomized, double-blind controlled studies suggest that gamma-tACS improves overall cognitive function and memory performance [9]. Mechanistic explanations for the apparent benefit have included restoration of cholinergic neurotransmission, increased brain perfusion [10], and amyloid and tau clearance [11, 12].

A challenge for tACS is the generation of effects within deep brain structures, especially the hippocampus [13]. In nonhuman primates, conventional two scalp electrode tACS influences the spiking time of single neurons in deep brain structures, including the hippocampus [14]. Recently, our team directly recorded local field potentials (LFP) in the hippocampus in patients with drug-resistant epilepsy with stereoelectroencephalography (SEEG) electrodes and demonstrated that compared to 0 mA, greater than 7 mA tACS were required to produce significant differences in LEP in the hippocampus [15]. Additionally, it was observed that the LFPs increased significantly with higher external currents, especially at 15 mA. Most importantly, no seizure activity was observed in patients during tACS, indicating that 15 mA may offer more effective stimulation compared to lower currents, while remaining safe. These research data suggest that tACS can be proposed for neuromodulation of deep brain structures. However, we do not yet know the nature of changes of oscillations in the hippocampus after tACS and its potential to improve cognitive function in AD.

We conducted this randomized, sham-controlled, clinical trial with the primary objective to determine the effect of 40 Hz tACS on global cognitive functioning of patients with mild dementia due to AD with evidence of amyloid pathology obtained either through CSF or amyloid PET scan. As a secondary objective, we used simultaneous electroencephalography–functional magnetic resonance imaging (EEG-fMRI) to determine the effects of tACS on hippocampal gamma activity.

Materials and methods

Study design

This single-center, double blind, randomized sham-controlled trial (ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT 03920826, Registration Date: 2019-04-19) [16] was conducted to examine the safety and effects of 30 one-hour sessions of 40 Hz tACS on cognitive function and hippocampal neural synchrony in mild AD. Ethical approval was received from Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University (2018-077) in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. We conducted this study following both the CONsolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) statement and the CONSORT statement for non-pharmacological interventions. The informed consent from all participants was obtained.

Participants

All participants met NIA-AA core clinical criteria for probable AD dementia [17] and the following additional inclusion criteria: (1) Literate in Han Chinese; (2) aged 45 to 75 years; (3) completed at least 6 years of education; (4) Global score of Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR) 1.0 [18]; (5) positive result for amyloid PET examination or low CSF Abeta42 [19]; (6) stable dose of cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine for > 6 weeks. Exclusion criteria included: (1) current or past history of any neurological disorder other than AD, such as epilepsy, stroke, progressive neurological disease (e.g., multiple sclerosis), poorly controlled migraines or intracranial brain lesions, and history of previous neurosurgery or head trauma that resulted in residual neurological impairment; (2) prescribed medication that might affect cognitive function such as anticonvulsants, antipsychotics, benzodiazepines, muscle relaxants, opiates, stimulants, and tricyclic antidepressants; (3) contraindications for undergoing MRI or receiving tACS, for example metal implants, pacemaker, claustrophobia and incomplete skulls; (4) eczema or sensitive skin; (5) abnormal brain structural MRI scan including hydrocephalus, stroke, structural lesions, significant white matter lesions (Fazekas score = 3–6), could potentially confound the outcome; (6) depression or other psychiatric disorders; (7) clinically significant gastrointestinal, renal, hepatic, respiratory, infectious, endocrine, or cardiovascular diseases, cancer, alcoholism or drug addiction.

Amyloid testing

PiB-PET imaging

PiB was synthesized and radiolabeled with 11 C following the method previously described [20]. Detailed scanning procedures are outlined in our protocol article [16]. A global cortical PiB-PET retention ratio was calculated by taking the median uptake across voxels in key regions of interest (prefrontal, orbitofrontal, parietal, temporal, anterior cingulate, and posterior cingulate/precuneus) and normalizing it to the cerebellar gray matter median. A retention ratio greater than 1.25 was considered positive.

CSF Aβ42 levels

CSF Aβ42 levels were measured using commercially available ELISA kits (Fujirebio, Ghent, Belgium), adhering strictly to the manufacturer’s instructions [21]. Results were reported as precise concentrations, calculated using standard curves established daily with six ready-to-use calibrators. Each measurement was validated with two quality control samples and two internal controls. The normal reference intervals for these CSF biomarkers varied by age group: 610–974 pg/mL (21–50 years), 562–1018 pg/mL (51–70 years), and 567–1027 pg/mL (> 70 years). An Aβ42 level below the lower limit for the respective age group was considered positive.

Randomization and masking

Participants were randomly assigned 1:1 to tACS or sham stimulation. An independent statistician generated the randomization sequence and coded the tACS device. The random allocation was performed using the random number table method in SAS software (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC). Trained nurses administered the tACS, opening an opaque, sealed envelope containing each participant’s group allocation code before the first intervention. A single experienced psychological assessor, blinded to the group allocation, conducted all patient cognitive assessments. Participants, caregivers, outcomes assessors, and nurses were all blinded to randomization status. The details are shown in the published protocol and supplemental files (page 1–12, statistical analysis plan).

Procedures

To better observe and manage potential adverse reactions, all participants were admitted to the ward and received interventions during their hospitalization. A NEXALIN ADI transcranial alternating current stimulator was used to give 40 Hz stimulation; peak-to-peak amplitude 15 mA tACS [22]. A 4.45 × 9.53 cm rectangular pad was positioned on the forehead, aligning with Fpz, Fp1, and Fp2 of the international placement system. Meanwhile, two 3.18 × 3.81 cm rectangular pads were affixed behind each ear over the mastoid region. The location of the electrodes and the computational modelling of the electric fields from tACS are shown in the appendix (Figure S1).

Participants received 30 one-hour sessions of tACS or sham tACS across consecutive 15 days (twice a day, intervals of at least 4 h). At baseline, the end of the intervention, and three months after randomization, all participants underwent a cognitive testing battery, structural MRI, and simultaneous EEG-fMRI. We collected EEG and EEG-fMRI data while participants were in a resting state. The study schedule of the trial is shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Schedule of the trial

Outcomes

The primary outcome was the change in the scores of the 11-item cognitive subscale of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-Cog) [23] from the baseline to the end of intervention. To reduce the potential practice effect, an alternate list of words was used for the Word Recall Task (Item 1) at the end of the intervention, and the original list was used at baseline and at the 3-month follow-up. Secondary outcomes included: the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [24]; the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [25]; the World Health Organization-University of California-Los Angeles auditory verbal learning test (WHO-UCLA AVLT) [26]; digit span; the trail-making test (TMT) B minus A score; the Boston Naming Test (BNT); the activities of daily living (ADL) scale; the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) [27]; the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [28]; changes in EEG indices, including gamma frequency power and intensity of theta-gamma coupling in the hippocampus; and changes in the neural activity of the hippocampus.

T1-weighted images of the whole brain were acquired using a sagittal three-dimensional (3D) magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence. EEG data were collected simultaneously with fMRI using an MRI-compatible 64-channel (Ag/AgCl) electrode cap. Detailed description of EEG/fMRI data acquisition and data preprocessing was included in the supplementary materials.

In this study, adverse events were assessed after every five sessions. Every participant was asked about the presence and severity of the following symptoms: headache, neck pain, scalp pain, tingling, itching, ringing/buzzing noise, burning sensations, skin redness, sleepiness, trouble concentrating, acute mood change, dizziness, flickering lights, and any other symptoms.

EEG analysis

Details of EEG preprocessing could be found in the supplemental methods. For each EEG channel, spectral power was calculated for gamma band (30–50 Hz). Channels were grouped into four zones (Fig. 1a left; Figure S2a left). The left frontal zone included F1, F3, F5, F7, FC1, FC5, and FT7. The right frontal zone included F2, F4, F6, F8, FC2, FC4, FC6, and FT8. The left posterior zone included P1, P3, P5, P7, PO3, PO5, and PO7. The right posterior zone included P2, P4, P6, P8, PO4, PO6, and PO8. Statistical comparison of spectral power between different sessions was implemented for each zone.

Volume source localization for signals from the hippocampus was implemented using Brainstorm [29]. The head model for each participant was computed by OpenMEEG boundary element method (BEM) [30, 31] based on their T1 image and the corresponding Freesurfer output. Then EEG signals were inversed using dSPM method [32]. Sources in each side of the hippocampus were extracted and averaged using Desikan-Killiany Atlas [33]. Phase amplitude coupling within left and right hippocampus was calculated as mean vector length (MVL) modulation index [34], implemented by PACTools, an EEGLAB plug-in. MVL modulation indices were normalized by non-parametric permutation (n = 1000).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Increase of gamma power in EEG channels from baseline to the end of the intervention. (a) The distribution of the gamma power change in the sham group and the tACS group is shown. Channels are grouped into four regions: left frontal, right frontal, left posterior, and right posterior regions, shown by the dotted lines. (b) The change of gamma power in both tACS group and sham group are presented. Error bars represent the 95% confidence interval

* uncorrected p < 0.05. Those results were not significant after false discovery rate correction

fMRI data analysis

Details of fMRI preprocessing could be found in the supplemental methods. fMRI-based functional connectivity analysis was carried out using CONN toolbox (version 21b) [35]. Preprocessed dataset from fMRIPrep was imported and then denoised. Specifically, noise components from white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal areas (CSF), motion, outlier scans, and session effects were regressed out. Temporal band-pass filtering (0.008–0.09 Hz) was applied. Since it has been shown that the hippocampus actually includes a series of distinct and interacting subregions by previous studies [36, 37], which could be influenced differently by AD [38]. To see whether tACS intervention also impact hippocampus subregions differently, the hippocampus was partitioned into head, body, and tail by FreeSurfer [39]. To measure whether the intervention improved the regional activity in different subregions of the hippocampus, average fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (fALFF) was calculated in the subregions of the hippocampus. fALFF, as the ratio of low-frequency power to the full-range power, is thought to improve the sensitivity and specificity on detecting changes of regional brain activity [40].

EEG-fMRI data analysis

To study the association between gamma power fluctuation and hippocampus neural activation, the fluctuation of gamma power (30–50 Hz) in scalp EEG channels was calculated using EEGLAB with a window size of two seconds, which corresponds to the sampling rate of fMRI BOLD signal. A regression analysis was carried out with the fluctuation of gamma power as the regressor and the average BOLD signals in the subregions of the hippocampus as the response. The changes of beta values (regression coefficients) between sessions were calculated.

Statistical analyses

The sample size was calculated based on the results of our preliminary experiments, in which ADAS-Cog scores improved after 30 sessions of tACS intervention (the changes of the scores in tACS group vs. control group: mean ± SD: -3.12 ± 2.35 vs. -0.67 ± 2.13). To allow for a maximum dropout rate of 20%, we aimed to recruit and randomize 40 participants to provide 83.9% power to obtain a statistically significant group difference with a two-sided α level of 0.05.

Data were analyzed according to intention-to-treat principles. SAS 9.4 statistical software (SAS Institute, license number: 11202165) was utilized for the analysis. Independent sample t-tests for continuous data and chi-square tests for dichotomous variables (Fisher’s exact tests, if needed) were used to explore the baseline characteristics of our participants. For the neuropsychological assessment data, we first performed a Shapiro-Wilk test to check for normality. For data that followed a normal distribution, we used a mixed-effects model for analysis. Group, time, and group*time interactions were fixed effects, with intercept as the random term. We utilized two distinct linear mixed-effects models. The first model evaluated changes from baseline to the end of the intervention, while the second examined the differences between baseline and the 3-month follow-up. For data that did not follow a normal distribution, we used the Mann-Whitney U non-parametric test on the pre-post intervention difference.

In EEG analysis, the changes of gamma power after intervention in each group were tested by one-sample t-tests. The group comparison between sham and tACS groups in the changes of theta-gamma coupling were tested by two-sample independent t-tests. The correlation between the changes of theta-gamma coupling and the changes of MMSE and delayed recall scores was evaluated using Pearson’s r. In fMRI analysis, the changes of fALFF and the association between BOLD signal and gamma power fluctuation after intervention were tested by one-sample t-tests.

Results

Participants and the changes of cognitive functions

Between September 12, 2019, and December 27, 2021, 112 individuals were screened for inclusion; 66 were excluded (Fig. 2). Table 2 displays the baseline characteristics of participants.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The flowchart of our study

Table 2 Baseline characteristics of the two groups

Table 3 presents the changes in outcome measure scores. Analysis of the primary outcome showed that there was no statistically significant improvement in the ADAS-Cog score with tACS at the completion of treatment (group*time, p = 0.449, mixed-effect models) or at 3-month follow-up (group*time, p = 0.739, mixed-effect models). Among the secondary outcomes, compared to the control group, the intervention group showed significant improvement in MMSE (group*time, p = 0.041) and MoCA scores (non-parametric test, p = 0.025) (Table 3). However, this effect was not sustained at the 3-month follow-up. No statistically significant positive results were observed for other neuropsychological tests.

Table 3 Estimated mean change in the primary outcome and in other neuropsychological assessment

EEG analysis

For EEG analysis, channels were clustered into left frontal, right frontal, left posterior, and right posterior channels (Fig. 1a left). There was significant increase of gamma power (30–50 Hz) in left frontal (t = 2.37, p = 0.032, t-tests), left posterior (t = 2.68, p = 0.017, t-tests), and right posterior (t = 2.13, p = 0.049, t-tests) regions in the tACS group (Fig. 1b). However, those effects did not reach significance after false discovery rate (FDR) correction. No change of gamma power was observed in the sham group (ps > 0.05, t-tests). There were also no significant changes in gamma power from baseline to 3-month follow-up in either group (all ps > 0.05; t-tests, Figure S2).

In the left hippocampus, coupling between 5 and 6 Hz phase and 30–42 Hz amplitude increased statistically significantly from baseline to the end of intervention by comparing the tACS group and the sham group (two-sample t test, ps < 0.05; Fig. 3a). Within this range, we found a statistically significant correlation between the change of theta-gamma coupling in the left hippocampus and the change of MMSE (5.5 –40.5 Hz: r = 0.34, p = 0.049; 5.5 –41.5 Hz: r = 0.37, p = 0.029; Pearson’s r, Fig. 3b left). There was no statistically significant change in theta-gamma coupling in the right hippocampus. However, we found a statistically significant correlation between post-intervention theta-gamma coupling in the right hippocampus and post-intervention delayed recall score in the tACS group (6.5–30 Hz: r = 0.51, p = 0.044; Pearson’s r, Fig. 3b right), but not the in the sham group (6.5–30 Hz: r = -0.16, p = 0.519, Pearson’s r). This correlation was not observed in the baseline session of the tACS group (6.5–30 Hz: r = 0.20, p = 0.462, Pearson’s r). No statistically significant difference in theta-gamma coupling changes was observed between the intervention group and the control group in the hippocampus from baseline to the 3-months follow-up session (all ps > 0.05; Figure S3, t-tests).

Fig. 3
figure 3

EEG source signals in the hippocampus. Due to data quality issue, only a part of the participants was included in the EEG analysis (n = 34). (a) The changes of theta-gamma coupling from baseline to the end of the intervention were contrasted between the tACS group (n = 16) and the sham group (n = 18). The results are displayed for the left and right hippocampus within the range of theta phase (4–8 Hz) and gamma power (30–50 Hz). (b) A statistically significant correlation was observed between the change of theta-gamma coupling at 5.5–41.5 Hz and the change of MMSE (left, n = 34). There was also a statistically significant correlation between the theta-gamma coupling at 6.5–30 Hz and the delayed recall score in the tACS group (n = 16) at the end of intervention (right)

TGC, theta-gamma coupling

fMRI analysis

We calculated fALFF, based on BOLD signal in the left and right hippocampus of the tACS group. Each side of the hippocampus was split into head, body and tail. We found a statistically significant increase of fALFF after intervention in the right hippocampus head (t = 2.20, p = 0.039; t-tests, Fig. 4a). We also regressed BOLD signals in the hippocampus by gamma power fluctuation in the channels with most statistically significant increase of gamma power (F3, F7, PO3, PO7, PO8). We found increased association between BOLD signals in the left and right hippocampus tails and gamma power fluctuation in PO8 from baseline to the end of intervention in the tACS group (left hippocampus tail: t = 2.14, p = 0.049; right hippocampus tail: t = 2.56, p = 0.022; Fig. 4b). No statistically significant differences in fMRI results were seen in the hippocampus from the baseline to 3-month follow-up (Figure S4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

The changes of fMRI outcomes. (a) The changes of fALFF in the left and right hippocampus in the tACS group. (b) The changes of coefficients derived from regression of hippocampus BOLD signals by gamma power fluctuation in PO8 channel in the tACS group. fALFF, fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation

* p < 0.05

Adverse events

All participants underwent intervention while hospitalized, allowing for continuous monitoring by doctors for adverse reactions. This arrangement also allowed participants to promptly report any concerns to doctors. To ensure that no potential side effects went unnoticed, we administered a questionnaire following every five sessions. This helped in tracking side effects and acted as a reminder for patients to be alert to any symptoms. During the course of the study, one patient in the tACS group reported a tingling sensation during the first two sessions, while another in the control group reported transient itching at the first session. No serious adverse events were observed in either group.

Discussion

Earlier studies have shown cognitive improvement following single or multi-session 40 Hz tACS in patients with AD [9]. Our study failed to show effects of 40 Hz tACS on the primary outcome of global cognition as measured by ADAS-Cog. However, effects on the secondary outcome measure of delayed recall supports the hypothesis that tACS may improve more specific cognitive functions, including episodic memory, that can be confirmed in subsequent studies. We found that tACS enhanced theta-gamma coupling in the hippocampus of patients with AD. This increase in gamma oscillation synchrony in the hippocampus was correlated with the improvement of MMSE and delayed recall. Our study also showed that tACS increased local neural activity in the hippocampus as measured by low frequency fluctuations in fMRI, providing additional evidence for effects of tACS on the hippocampus.

tACS has been shown to induce whole-brain effects, with studies suggesting the entrainment of neurons across widespread cortical regions [41]. This implies that even weak stimulation could induce large-scale modulation of neural activity via network resonance [10, 41]. Within our study, using computational modelling, we demonstrated that electric stimulation fields reached widespread brain regions. We also found gamma power was increased in multiple regions, including frontal and parietal-occipital lobes, not limited to the stimulation areas. Thus, tACS is capable of producing changes in whole-brain activity in people with AD. In this study, we did not detect statistically significant improvements in the ADAS-Cog, however we did find statistically significant improvement in MMSE and MoCA score. Although we had a priori power calculation and reached the calculated sample size, it is still possible that the small sample size used in the trial may have obscured benefits that would have been demonstrable with the several hundreds of participants currently used to detect modest benefits in AD treatment trials. Our data indicating good safety and acceptability, together with positive results for some outcomes and consistent change in neurophysiological measures, would support the utility of launching a phase 3 trial, provided it is adequately powered to detect small treatment benefits on overall cognitive functioning and activities of daily living. We believe that there were several factors influencing the response to tACS intervention. For instance, in a previous study, ApoE genotype and baseline cognitive impairment were identified as predictors of response to tACS [42]. We plan to conduct a subgroup analysis in our subsequent study to further identify such predictors of response.

Among secondary outcomes, we found that memory function improved statistically significantly after the intervention. Two previous randomized controlled studies have shown that tACS can improve memory function by stimulating the Pz and precunus, both of which are key nodes in the default model network (DMN) [42, 43]. In addition to using the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning (RAVL) to assess episodic memory, these two studies also used the Face-Naming Association memory task (FNAT) to assess associative memory, where performance has been suggested to depend primarily on the hippocampus. Although these studies did not directly measure neural activity in the hippocampus, they speculated that the improvement in the FNAT after tACS is due to the functional connection between the DMN network and the hippocampus. Thus, although we had aimed to use an ADAS-Cog as an outcome that was comparable with other clinical trials, in future studies we would recommend using a cognitive outcome that is closer to the assumed mechanism of action of the intervention, i.e. memory consolidation. Here, indeed we found a statistically significant effect of tACS on delayed recall, underscoring a potential mechanistic effect of tACS on memory.

Unlike superficial brain areas such as the Pz and precuneus, the hippocampus is challenging to stimulate. Current studies on the effects of tACS on hippocampal neuron activity in AD are still insufficient, although it should be mentioned that one study suggested that tACS stimulation of the temporal lobe region increased blood flow in the hippocampus in AD [10], and a recently published study showed that 40 Hz tACS increased the functional connectivity between hippocampus and inferior parietal lobe [44]. Our prior study using the same tACS device suggested greater than 7 mA tACS were required to produce significant LEP in the hippocampus and a 15 mA electrical stimulation for one hour with this device is safe and did not induce seizure activity [15]. Our study initially used computational modelling to show that electrical current can reach the hippocampus. Then, through EEG-fMRI regression, we demonstrated an increased association between neural activity in the hippocampus and gamma power fluctuation in the intervention group. This suggested that the increased gamma power recorded from scalp channels was at least partly due to enhanced activity of the hippocampus. We analyzed neural activity in the hippocampus using EEG source localization for signals in the hippocampus and found that theta-gamma coupling was enhanced in the hippocampus, which correlated with MMSE improvement and scores for delayed recall. fMRI analyses further showed an increase in local hippocampal BOLD signals. These results indicate that tACS can reach and influence the hippocampus, modulating neuronal activity in this region. In the entorhinal-hippocampal system, theta-gamma coupling is observed specifically in the slow-gamma frequency [45]. As the difficulty of cognitive task is increased, theta-slow gamma coupling is augmented, suggesting that theta-gamma coupling reflects a compensatory mechanism to maintain memory function [46]. Our study suggests that the improvements in cognitive function of patients with AD after tACS are associated with effects on theta-gamma coupling in the hippocampus.

The hippocampus is a critical brain region for cognitive function, particularly episodic memory. However, current researches on the lateralization of hippocampal function remain inconclusive. Regarding verbal memory function, some studies suggest that the left hippocampus is more relevant [47, 48], while others indicate that the volume of the right hippocampus is associated with delayed verbal recall [49]. Our study found that delayed verbal recall improved after intervention and was associated with an increase in theta-gamma coupling in the right hippocampus after intervention, along with an increase in fALFF values in the right hippocampus. The different findings in the left and right hippocampi in our study may be related to the lateralization of hippocampal functions, but it could also be due to the small sample size, leading to less comprehensive results. Therefore, our exploratory study suggests that tACS can alter neuronal activity in the hippocampus, but whether the intervention effects are lateralized requires further research for verification. Additionally, regarding the subregion analysis of the hippocampus, we found significant increase of fALFF in right hippocampus head after intervention. According to previous studies, the anterior hippocampus is involved in the encoding of episodic memory, while the posterior hippocampus participates in the decoding of episodic memory [50, 51]. We suspected that the tACS intervention might enhance memory encoding by activating the hippocampus head. On the other hand, the association between BOLD signals and gamma power fluctuation increased after intervention in the tail of both left and right hippocampus. In other words, gamma-band activity account for more brain activation in the hippocampus tail, which might increase the efficiency of memory retrieval.

Any improvements in cognitive function and changes in hippocampal neuronal activity that we observed post-intervention were not evident at the 3-month follow-up. A recent systematic review of tACS for MCI and dementia [9] indicated that most tACS interventions are single-session, suggesting that tACS can exhibit rapid effects. Published studies suggest that repeated stimulation increases gamma power, but this diminishes upon cessation of stimulation [12]. One study examined the long-term effects of tACS and found that 30 sessions improved MMSE and ADAS-Cog scores, but the ADAS-COG scores returned to baseline at the 12-week follow-up [11].

Limitations

We acknowledge a number of study limitations. The sample size was relatively small. And although we found that the effects of stimulation could reach and affect the hippocampus, we are still not sure if the stimulation locations are optimal for treating AD. Another point to note is that, for safety consideration, the participants we enrolled are relatively younger compared to those in other studies [10, 42, 43]. Currently, there is a lack of research on the effectiveness of tACS intervention in AD across different age groups. However, some studies have explored the impact of age on responsiveness to non-invasive stimulation paradigms delivered to the motor cortex, including tACS [52,53,54]. These studies suggested that increasing age may reduce responsiveness to stimulation. However, the considerable heterogeneity in the findings makes it difficult to draw definitive conclusions. Therefore, further research is needed to analyze the impact of age on the responsiveness of AD patients to tACS interventions. In the present study, some of the EEG-fMRI results did not pass correction for multiple comparisons. Furthermore, we did not identify a substantial group difference between the treatment group and the sham group for the change in gamma power. These null results may be due to the relatively small sample size, which prevented sufficient statistical power. Therefore, we consider our results to be exploratory and preliminary. We hope that our study can provide a foundation for future larger studies to validate our results. Lastly, at the 3-month follow-up, there were 2 vs. 5 participants missing, which may impact the results of the 3-month evaluation. The lasting effects of tACS on AD need further clarification in subsequent studies.

Conclusion

In conclusion, multi-session 40 Hz tACS over the forehead and two mastoid regions is safe and well tolerated and has potential to improve general cognitive function and delayed memory in mild AD. Our results suggest that enhancement of theta-gamma coupling and neural activity of the hippocampus is a potential therapeutic mechanism of tACS.