Background

Intra-specific contests, primarily over resources and reproductive opportunities, are a ubiquitous aspect of animal behaviour. Such contests are highly significant as they can determine a large proportion (or indeed all) of an animal's lifetime fitness, and pose the risk of injury or death when they escalate to fights. Because of these risks, contest resolution is often achieved without fighting, through processes of mutual and/or self-assessment [13]. However, models of animal contest predict that escalated contests leading to fatal fighting become more likely when the outcome of a contest represents a greater proportion of lifetime fitness [1, 4]. That is, when contestants have little chance of fitness gains outside of the contest in question, fights should occur regardless of the risks involved because there is much to gain and little or nothing to lose [1, 5]. Furthermore, kinship between contestants will not prevent escalated contests as long as the ratio between the value of the future and the value of the contest is small [4, 5]. Fatal fighting can arise when Vo/V < 1 - r, where Vo and V are respectively the value of the future and of the current contest in terms of lifetime fitness, and r is the coefficient of relatedness between contestants [4]. Grafen [5] termed this the 'desperado effect', to describe the point where respect for conventions and asymmetries between contestants breaks down.

Escalated fighting occurs between relatives in a 'desperado' context in several circumstances: among male fig wasps and ants over mating opportunities [69], among nursery siblings over access to food resources in birds and mammals when young are overproduced [10, 11], and among social insect queens over resource inheritance [3, 12]. Contests are of utmost significance among queens of social insects as, in many species, colonies of potentially thousands of individuals are headed by one reproductive queen. Becoming queen thus represents a potentially huge fitness payoff, because once victorious, a queen is insulated from further environmental risks by the workers and can live for many years. Contests over the queen role can occur in normally monogynous species when several queens temporarily co-occur in one nest, such as during queen replacement, during colony reproduction via fission, and in pleometrotic foundress associations. In each case monogyny is soon restored: excess queens are culled by workers or killed during direct fighting between queens [12]. In such situations, there are usually very limited or no other reproductive opportunities for candidate queens. Thus, we would expect strong conflict between queens over colony inheritance to give rise to escalated fighting, and indeed this occurs in a range of species with variable degrees of relatedness between contestants (eg: honeybees [13, 14], bumblebees [15], ants [1618] and wasps [19]).

In the ant Aphaenogaster senilis, contests over colony inheritance occur during emergency queen replacement following orphaning. A few full-sister replacement queens are produced and the first-born queen is usually behaviourally dominant over later emerging queens [20]. Late-born queens apparently cede to the first-born queen without fighting and are eventually killed by workers. In experimental groups with two queens, second-born queens did not fight and usually died, even when the first-born had one mandible ablated and could not bite effectively [21]. Alternative reproductive opportunities for late-born queens are almost non-existent, and thus ceding to the first-born is effectively a form of suicide [21]. This lack of reciprocal fighting apparently contradicts the predictions of contest models.

However, models based on hawk-dove games generally assume that in an encounter between two hawks, one will survive while the other is killed. This does not take into account the possibility that both contestants die and thus obtain zero inclusive fitness. In do-or-die contests such as those over colony inheritance, it is possible that the victor sustains a mortal injury during the fight, and subsequently dies or is killed by workers, leading to the death of both contestants [eg: [21]]. Furthermore, as replacement queens are reared from existing queen-laid eggs in most social hymenoptera (workers typically cannot produce female brood), further queens cannot be reared once existing brood are exhausted, and there is a risk of colony death through failure to requeen.

A queen that cedes and is killed can still gain indirect fitness though the victorious queen if they are related, while at the same time avoiding the risk of a zero fitness outcome. Thus, the risk of both contestants dying may impose a significant cost to fighting if relatedness between the contestants is high [21]. Highly related contestants in 'desperado' contests, such as late-born A. senilis queens, may secure more indirect fitness by not fighting than direct fitness from victory when the latter is devalued by the risk of both queens dying [21]. Here, we formalise this reasoning by developing a simple model to explain how not fighting can be selectively advantageous in a 'desperado' contest between relatives. We focus on 'desperado' situations, where contestant's alternative options for fitness gain are negligible or absent. The area of contests outside of this has been explored by a suite of reproductive skew models (reviewed in [22]). Our model was developed to explain behaviour observed in social insect colonies, and we initially focus on these systems. The applicability of the rationale to other taxa is explored in the discussion.

Life history of A. senilis

A. senilis is a monogynous, monandrous ant common to the Mediterranean basin, that reproduces by colony fission: queens are born with short wings and cannot fly, and only disperse with groups of workers on foot [23, 24]. Colonies consist of around 1,300 workers and are monodomous. Colonies temporarily contain multiple queens when new queens are reared during preparation for colony fission or when the queen has died and must be replaced. Because of haplodiploidy, these new queens are full-sisters and are related by 0.75. The frequency of requeening in the field is unknown, but orphaning in the lab results in the production of one to six replacement queens: in one experiment 30 orphaned colonies produced 2.0 ± 1.1 queens [20], and in a second experiment 60 orphaned groups produced 2.8 ± 1.3 queens each [21]. New queens are produced 14-17 days apart on average. Only one queen survives, and it is usually the first-born who inherits the colony while subsequently born queens are killed [20, 21].

Many strictly monogynous social insects go through transient polygynous phases: pleometrosis (founding of nests by multiple queens) occurs in many species as a regular mode of colony initiation [25], and fission occurs in a wide range of species, some of which are monogynous (eg: honeybees and army ants [26]). The frequency of queen replacement in nature is largely unknown, but it may be common in species that reproduce via fission (Cronin et al. unpublished data). Obligate monogyny in social insects is widespread, despite the fact that polygyny can be favoured for a variety of reasons including increased colony productivity, increased colony longevity, and high mortality of dispersing queens [[27], pp 126-134].

Findings

The model

We define our contest arena as a nest containing two queens and associated workers. This matches the experimental data on A. senilis, where the majority of contests during queen replacement are between two queens [20]. We assume monogyny and thus all but one queen will be killed. Reduction in queen number takes place via direct conflict between the queens if either queen initiates a contest, or by worker culling of one queen if neither queen initiates a fight. Queens cannot gain direct fitness by avoiding contest because queen selection will occur regardless of whether queens fight or not, and because queens have no reproductive opportunities outside the contest for colony ownership (thus Vo = 0; Table 1).

Table 1 Abbreviations and variables used in the model.

For simplicity, we consider the contest in isolation, without regard to strategies employed in the rest of the population. We assume that the winning queen gains equal fitness (V) whether a fight occurs or not, as the duration of conflict (a few days) is negligible compared to the length of time fitness is gained (a few years), and non lethal injuries sustained during a fight have no effect on reproductive value since the queens accrue resources indirectly through workers. The effect of relaxing these assumptions is discussed later. We further assume that workers select queens based on some criteria of asymmetry between queens, as is common in species where intra-specific contests arise (eg: order of arrival or age [20, 28, 29]). This asymmetry gives one competitor some advantage, and the queen which holds the advantage in the asymmetry (Q1) will win the contest if no fighting occurs (workers will kill the other queen). The advantage may be uncorrelated with the individual's intrinsic quality [30], and we consider it independently of fighting ability and reproductive value. In A. senilis, the asymmetry is determined by birth order, and the first born queen (Q1) generally inherits. How workers select one queen over the other is generally unclear, but may be related to a need to expedite the queen replacement process [20, 21], or because of queen manipulation [31, 32].

The possible contest outcomes are thus a) Q1 inherits the colony, either through killing Q2, or when Q2 is killed by workers in the case that no queen-queen fight occurs, b) Q2 inherits the colony by killing Q1, or c) both queens are killed fighting. Queens can gain direct fitness through inheriting the colony or indirect fitness when a relative inherits the colony. The fitness payoff for a given queen depends on the probability of winning the fight if they do fight, the risk of both queens dying, and the genetic relatedness between the queens.

Both queens may either choose to fight or refrain from fighting, and we assume a fight occurs if one queen initiates fighting. Q1 obtains a direct fitness of V if there is no fighting (workers kill Q2; see Table 1 for definitions of variables used in the model). When fighting occurs, Q1 has an inclusive fitness of:

( a V + ( 1 a ) r V ) ( 1 d )
(1)

Where a is the probability of Q1 winning an overt fight and 1 - a is the probability of Q2 winning. Genetic relatedness between contestants is denoted by r, and d represents the probability of a zero fitness outcome because both queens are killed in the contest.

From the above, it can be seen that Q1 prefers not to fight when:

V > ( a V + ( 1 a ) r V ) ( 1 d )
(2)

Q2 obtains no direct fitness if she elects not to fight (workers kill her), but she obtains an indirect fitness of r V. If Q2 fights, her inclusive fitness is (a r V + (1 - a) V) (1 - d). Thus, Q2 favours not fighting when:

r V > ( a r V + ( 1 a ) V ) ( 1 d )
(3)

Solving these inequalities yields the values for which not fighting is favoured by Q1 {2} and Q2 {3}. From {2} it is clear that Q1 never prefers fighting, which is intuitive as she stands to inherit if there is no fighting. For Q2, the decision to fight or not is independent of the value of the contest (V) but dependent on the probability of winning (a), relatedness (r) and the probability of a zero fitness outcome (d). Q2 prefers not to fight when:

d > ( 1 r ) ( 1 a ) / ( 1 a + a r )
(4)

When queens are close relatives, have developed in the same environment, and have been fed equally, they may have an equal probabilities of winning a fight (i.e. a = 0.5), in which case {4} becomes d >(1 - r)/(1 + r). This relationship is depicted in Fig. 1 and indicates that when queens are full sisters (r = 0.75), as in A. senilis, Q2 fights only if the probability of both queens dying is low (d < 1/7 (= 0.143)).

Figure 1
figure 1

Conditions favouring not fighting by Q2 for contestants of equal fighting ability. Critical values of d for which not fighting (above the line) or fighting (below the line) is favoured, for all values of r, from equation {4}. When fighting abilities are equal (a = ½), the decision whether to fight or not is a function of d and r. This figure indicates that Q2 increasingly favours not fighting for increasing values of r and d. When contestants are full sisters (r = 0.75), Q2 will not fight unless the risk of both queens dying is very low (d < 0.143).

However, fighting asymmetries between contestants are likely if size differences exist or if differences in eclosion time results in development related fighting advantages. We explore this possibility by defining the probability of Q1 winning an overt fight as a linear function of Q1's relative fighting ability f, with a(f) = ½ (f + 1). Equation {4} then becomes: d >(1 - f)/[1 - f + 2r/(1 - r)] (Fig. 2). In this scenario, Q2 prefers not to fight when relatedness is high, and fighting asymmetry has little impact on this preference. However, when relatedness is low the effect of the fighting asymmetry is markedly stronger when it is in favour of Q1: being the better fighter only marginally increases Q2's propensity to fight whereas being the poorer fighter can dramatically reduce it (for low values of r). As it is likely that any asymmetry is in favour of the first-born [eg: [21, 33]], increasing fighting asymmetry can have the effect of reducing Q2's propensity to fight.

Figure 2
figure 2

Conditions favouring not fighting by Q2. Critical values of d for which not fighting (above the lines) or fighting (below the lines) is favoured by Q2 for various values of f, from equation {4}. Each line represents a value of relatedness (r = 0.1, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75 and 0.9, with r = 0.75 (full sisters) indicated by a thick line). For clone queens (r = 1), critical values are always 0, and not fighting is always favoured. Not fighting is favoured when relatedness is high or when Q1 is a better fighter.

Finally, it is worth considering that relative fighting ability affects not only the probability of winning the fight, but also the risk that both queens die: more evenly matched queens are more likely to have longer fights leading to a higher risk of both queens dying. In this case, the risk of both queens dying d becomes a diminishing function of increasing relative fighting ability. We can define d as a function of Q1's relative fighting ability f and a mortality index m, following d(f) = m e-αf². The variable m describes the lethality of fights in a given species while α defines the shape of the distribution (Table 1). Inequality {4} then becomes m >(1 - f)/[(1 - f + 2r/(1 - r))e-αf²]. Fig. 3 shows d(f) for α = 5, and the critical mortality above which Q2 does not fight. When queens have similar fighting abilities Q2 still prefers not to fight, but increasing disparity in fighting ability now increases Q2's propensity to fight, because the risk of a zero fitness outcome is reduced. Overall, Figs. 1 and 3 show that Q2 favours not fighting when there is a risk to her inclusive fitness: when the risk that both queens die from fighting is restricted to when queens have similar fighting abilities, the preference for not fighting by Q2 becomes similarly restricted. Thus, while the threat of a zero fitness outcome can favour not fighting by subordinate queens, large differences in fighting ability can negate this.

Figure 3
figure 3

Mortality risk curve for α = 5, and corresponding conditions favouring not fighting by Q2. Effect of relative fighting ability of Q1 (f) on diminishing the probability that overt fighting leads to the death of both contestants following d = m e-αf². The inset graph shows the distribution of the probability of both queens dying for m = 1 and α = 5. The risk of mortality of both queens is restricted to a smaller range of f values, and is close to zero for extremely biased fighting ability. The main figure shows that when the risk to inclusive fitness decreases with increasing asymmetry in fighting ability, Q2 is more likely to fight, particularly when it is a relatively strong fighter. Not fighting remains the preferred strategy when fighting represents a risk to inclusive fitness.

Discussion

Overt fighting can be expected in animal contests when the value of the contest is high relative to the value of the future [4] as contestants have nothing to lose. However, in contests between relatives there is something to lose, because fighting poses the risk of both contestants dying: in fighting with a related opponent, a losing individual can risk inclusive fitness loss by fatally injuring its competitor. Our results suggest that relatedness between contestants can lead to restraint under specific circumstances, even when not fighting means death and there are no alternative future reproductive opportunities. Our model indicates that this is possible when three conditions are met: i) contests are 'desperado' contests, with one winner and no alternative opportunities for fitness gains for the loser, ii) fighting poses a risk of mortality to the victor and, iii) contestants are highly related.

We have assumed that Q2 decides for herself how to act in a given situation. An alternative possibility is that her behaviour is manipulated by Q1 and/or workers, both of which benefit from Q1 inheriting if she is fit. It is unclear whether Q1 has the means of manipulating Q2 (eg: through chemical signals), but workers presumably do, as they control the amount of food Q2 receives as a larva, which may allow them to control her growth rate and hence delay her emergence, thereby advantaging Q1. Mechanisms of influencing offspring development and contest outcomes have also been described in vertebrates [33]. However, whether Q2 is manipulated or not in her decision to fight in A. senilis awaits investigation.

We employ the simplifying assumption of considering our contest in isolation, without regard to population-wide strategies. In reality, model parameters such as the value of the contest V are not independent of strategies employed by other colonies in the population, and this should be considered in a more comprehensive model. Furthermore, we assume a 2-queen contest arena and, while our model is applicable to most A. senilis colonies where only a few queens are produced, in some colonies Q1 may need to win multiple consecutive contests, and this may be the norm in other species. In such cases, an injury sustained in a fight will lower the relative fighting ability of the queen for the next fight. For instance, if Q1 wins the fight but is injured in the process she may have a lower fighting ability against a third queen. Nonetheless, unless Q3 is a markedly better fighter, she will still elect not to fight in order not to jeopardize her inclusive fitness (Fig. 3 with -0.25 < f <0). Conversely, if Q3 is a much stronger fighter (f < - 0.4) there is little risk that the queens will kill one another and Q3 will fight. However, in A. senilis late born queens do not fight even when Q1 has had one mandible ablated and cannot bite effectively (ie: f is close to -1) [21]. It may be that in this species, queens have not been selected to assess fighting abilities because queens are full-sisters and differ too little in fighting ability to ever make fighting a successful strategy for Q2 (Fig. 1).

Clearly, the conditions required for self sacrifice to occur are restrictive, and probably do not arise in most cases of animal contests because i) relatedness between contestants is insufficiently high or ii) there are other opportunities for fitness acquisition outside the focal contest. This includes contests among ant queens following pleometrosis: as foundresses are usually unrelated [25] overt fighting eventuates (eg: Lasius niger [16, 18], Messor pergandei [34], Azteca xanthacroa [35], Solenopsis wagneri (monogyne form) [12, 17]).

Conditions potentially giving rise to desperado contests between relatives can arise in several contexts in insects: among queens fighting over colony inheritance in monogynous species that can requeen or reproduce by colony fission (eg: A. senilis [20, 21] and honeybees [3], see also Table 2), and among wingless males of Cardiocondyla and Hypoponera ants, which may fight until only one remains in the colony and monopolises mating opportunities [6, 7, 36, 37]. However, the available evidence suggests overt fighting is usually the norm, possibly because relatedness between contestants is low as a result of polygyny and polyandry (Table 2).

Table 2 Examples of contests between relatives in a potential 'desperado' context in social insects.

Males also fight over females in many fig wasps [8, 9, 38], where the intensity of fights varies between species from causing no injury [9] to causing a high level of life-threatening injuries [38, 39]. Here, self-sacrifice is not expected because competing males are not highly related in many species [38] and males may avoid fights they are unlikely to win [39].

In vertebrates, siblings fight over food in some birds and mammals producing more young than can be reared [40]. However, in many cases these may not be true desperado contests as all young may be reared in exceptionally bountiful years, even in 'obligately' siblicidal species [11, 41]. Nonetheless, examples of overt fighting leading to siblicide exist for a range of avian species and some mammals [[41] and references therein], in which junior young are usually killed by senior siblings. Conditions favouring self-sacrifice may not arise because relatedness between vertebrate siblings (r = 0.5) is lower than for Hymenopteran full-sisters (r = 0.75) and, furthermore, development related asymmetries may be so extreme that the risk of contestants mortally injuring one another is negligible (ie: there is no risk to inclusive fitness), rendering self-sacrifice irrelevant. We predict that circumstances that can give rise to self-sacrifice are most likely to be found in monogynous and monandrous species of social insect that can requeen or that reproduce by colony fission, as relatedness is high and death of all but one contestant is inevitable. Stingless bees (Meliponini) fulfil these criteria, but queen-queen contests can be precluded by worker pre-emptively culling of queens [42] and details of queen-queen conflict are presently lacking. Closer investigation of contests in species where the specific conditions described arise could demonstrate further examples of self-sacrifice.