Advertisement

Biological Theory

, Volume 5, Issue 1, pp 18–30 | Cite as

The Evolution of Religion: How Cognitive By-Products, Adaptive Learning Heuristics, Ritual Displays, and Group Competition Generate Deep Commitments to Prosocial Religions

  • Scott Atran
  • Joseph Henrich
Article

Abstract

Understanding religion requires explaining why supernatural beliefs, devotions, and rituals are both universal and variable across cultures, and why religion is so often associated with both large-scale cooperation and enduring group conflict. Emerging lines of research suggest that these oppositions result from the convergence of three processes. First, the interaction of certain reliably developing cognitive processes, such as our ability to infer the presence of intentional agents, favors—as an evolutionary by-product—the spread of certain kinds of counterintuitive concepts. Second, participation in rituals and devotions involving costly displays exploits various aspects of our evolved psychology to deepen people’s commitment to both supernatural agents and religious communities. Third, competition among societies and organizations with different faith-based beliefs and practices has increasingly connected religion with both within-group prosociality and between-group enmity. This connection has strengthened dramatically in recent millennia, as part of the evolution of complex societies, and is important to understanding cooperation and conflict in today’s world.

Keywords

by-product hypothesis credibility enhancing displays cultural transmission cooperation group competition high gods minimally counterintuitive morality religion rise of civilization 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alcorta CS, Sosis R (2005) Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols: The evolution of religion as an adaptive complex. Hurnan Nature 1: 323–359.Google Scholar
  2. Allen-Arave W, Gurven M, Hill K (2008) Reciprocal altruism, rather than kin selection, maintains nepotistic food transfers on an Ache reservation. Evolution and Human Behavior 29: 305–318.Google Scholar
  3. Anderson B (1991) The Imagined Communities: Reflections on the Origin and Spread of Nationalism. London: Verso.Google Scholar
  4. Argyle M, Beit-Hallahmi B (2000) The Social Psychology of Religion. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  5. Atran S (1998) Folkbiology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 547–609.Google Scholar
  6. Atran S (2002) In Gods We Trust. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Atran S (2003) Genesis of suicide terrorism. Science 299: 1534–1539.Google Scholar
  8. Atran S (2010) Talking to the Enemy: Faith, Brotherhood, and the (Un)Making of Terrorists. New York: Ecco/Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  9. Atran S, Axelrod R, Davis R (2007) Sacred barriers to conflict resolution. Science 317: 1039–1040.Google Scholar
  10. Atran S, Medin DL (2008) The native mind and the cultural construction of nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  11. Atran S, Medin D, Ross N, Lynch E, Vapnarsky V, Ek’ EU, Coley J, Timura C, Baran M (2002) Folkecology, cultural epidemiology, and the spirit of the commons: A garden experiment in the Maya lowlands, 1991–2001. Current Anthropology 43: 421–150.Google Scholar
  12. Atran S, Norenzayan A (2004) Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 713–770.Google Scholar
  13. Atran S, Sperber D (1991) Learning without teaching. In: Culture, Schooling and Psychological Development (Tolchinsky-Landsmann L, ed), 39–55. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  14. Bargh JA, Chartrand TL (1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist 54: 462–479.Google Scholar
  15. Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  16. Barrett JL (1998) Cognitive constraints on Hindu concepts of the divine. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 37: 608–619.Google Scholar
  17. Barrett JL (2000) Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Science 4: 29–34.Google Scholar
  18. Barrett JL (2004) Why Would Anyone Believe in God? Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
  19. Barrett JL, Keil FC (1996) Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology 31: 219–247.Google Scholar
  20. Barrett JL, Nyhof MA (2001) Spreading nonnatural concepts. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1: 69–100.Google Scholar
  21. Beck W (1992) Aboriginal preparation of Cycads seeds in Australia. Economic Botany 46: 133–147.Google Scholar
  22. Bloom P, Veres C (1999) The perceived intentionality of groups. Cognition 71: B1–B9.Google Scholar
  23. Boyd R, Gintis H, Bowles S, Richerson PJ (2003) The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 100: 3531–3535.Google Scholar
  24. Boyd R, Richerson P (1988) The evolution of reciprocity in sizable groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology 132: 337–356.Google Scholar
  25. Boyd R, Richerson P (2002) Group beneficial norms can spread rapidly in a structured population. Journal of Theoretical Biology 215: 287–296.Google Scholar
  26. Boyd R, Richerson J, Henrich J (n.d.) Rapid cultural adaptation can facilitate the evolution of large-scale cooperation. Manuscript. University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
  27. Boyer P (2001) Religion Explained. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  28. Boyer P, Ramble C (2001) Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science 25: 535–564.Google Scholar
  29. Campbell J (1974) The Mythic Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Csibra G, Gergely G, Bíró S, Koós O, Brockbank M (1999) Goal attribution without agency cues. Cognition 72: 237–267.Google Scholar
  31. Currie TE, Mace R (2009) Political complexity predicts the spread of ethnolinguistic groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106: 7339–7344.Google Scholar
  32. Dehghani M, Iliev R, Sachdeva S, Atran S, Ginges J, Medin D (2009) Emerging sacred values: The Iranian nuclear program. Judgment and Decision Making 4: 990–993.Google Scholar
  33. Diesendruck G, Haber L (2009) God’s categories: The effect of religiosity on children’s teleological and essentialist beliefs about categories. Cognition 110: 100–114.Google Scholar
  34. Durkheim E (1995) Elementary Forms of Religious Life. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  35. Ensminger J (1997) Transaction costs and Islam: Explaining conversion in Africa. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics 153: 4–28.Google Scholar
  36. Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature 425: 785–791.Google Scholar
  37. Festinger L, Riecken H, Schachter S (1956) When Prophecy Fails. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  38. Firth R (1963) Offering and sacrifice. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 93: 12–24.Google Scholar
  39. Fishman A, Goldschmidt Y (1990) The orthodox kibbutzim and economic success. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 29: 505–511.Google Scholar
  40. Freston P (2001) Evangelicals and Politics in Asia, Africa and Latin America. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Gazzaniga MS, Ivry RB, Mangun GR (2009) Cognitive Neuroscience, 3rd ed. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  42. Gervais W, Henrich J (forthcoming) The Zeus problem: Why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods. Journal of Cognition and Culture.Google Scholar
  43. Ginges J, Atran S, Medin DL, Shikaki K (2007) Sacred bounds on rational resolution of violent political conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104: 7357–7360.Google Scholar
  44. Ginges J, Hansen I, Norenzayan A (2009) Religious and popular support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science 20: 224–230.Google Scholar
  45. Gintis H, Smith EA, Bowles S (2001) Costly signaling and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 213: 103–119.Google Scholar
  46. Greeley A (1975) The Sociology of the Paranormal. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  47. Guthrie S (1993) Faces in the Clouds. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Harper L, Sanders KM (1975) The effect of adults’ eating on young children’s acceptance of unfamiliar foods. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20: 206–214.Google Scholar
  49. Harris P, Pasquini ES, Duke S, Asscher JJ, Pons F (2006) Germs and angels: The role of testimony in young children’s ontology. Developmental Science 9: 76–96.Google Scholar
  50. Heider F, Simmel S (1944) An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology 57: 243–259.Google Scholar
  51. Henrich J (2004) Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 53: 3–35.Google Scholar
  52. Henrich J (2008) A cultural species. In: Explaining Culture Scientifically (Brown M, ed), 184–210. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
  53. Henrich J (2009) The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior 30: 244–260.Google Scholar
  54. Henrich J, Boyd R (2001) Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology 208: 79–89.Google Scholar
  55. Henrich J, Boyd R (2002) On modeling cognition and culture: Why replicators are not necessary for cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2: 87–112.Google Scholar
  56. Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H, McElreath R, Alvard M, Barr A, Ensminger J, Hill K, Gil-White F, Gurven M, Marlowe F, Patton JQ, Smith N, Tracer D (2005) “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 795–815.Google Scholar
  57. Henrich J, Boyd R, Richerson P (2008) Five misunderstandings about cultural evolution. Human Nature 10: 253–289.Google Scholar
  58. Henrich J, Ensminger J, McElreath R, Barr A, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas JC, Gurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N, Lesorogol C, Marlowe F, Tracer F, Ziker J (2010) Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327: 1480–1484.Google Scholar
  59. Henrich J, Gil-White F (2001) The evolution of prestige. Evolution and Human Behavior 22: 165–196.Google Scholar
  60. Henrich J, McElreath R (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 123–135.Google Scholar
  61. Henrich NS, Henrich J (2007) Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Horner V, Whiten A (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees and children. Animal Cognition 8: 164–181.Google Scholar
  63. Hruschka D, Henrich J (2006) Friendship, cliquishness, and the emergence of cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 239: 1–15.Google Scholar
  64. Irons W (1996) In our own self-image. Skeptic 4(2): 50–52.Google Scholar
  65. Johnson D (2005) God’s punishment and public goods. Human Nature 16: 410–446.Google Scholar
  66. Johnson G (1987) In the name of the fatherland: An analysis of kin term usage in patriotic speech and literature. International Political Science Review 8: 165–174.Google Scholar
  67. Kapogiannis D, Barbey AK, Su M, Zamboni G, Krueger F, Grafman J (2009) Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106: 4876–4881.Google Scholar
  68. Keil F (1979) Semantic and Conceptual Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  69. Keleman D (2004) Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science 15: 295–301.Google Scholar
  70. Khaldûn I (2005) The Muqaddimah: An Introduction to History. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  71. Klein RG (1989) The Human Career: Human Biological and Cultural Origins. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  72. Kluckholn C, Leighton D (1946) The Navajo. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  73. Lee R (1979) The !Kung San: Men, Women and Work in a Foraging Society. New York: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
  74. Levy R (1957) The Social Structure of Islam. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  75. Liebenberg L (1990) The Art of Tracking. Cape Town, South Africa: David Philip Publishers.Google Scholar
  76. Lyons DE, Young AG, Keil FC (2007) The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104: 19751–19756.Google Scholar
  77. Malley B (2004) How the Bible Works: An Anthropological Study of Evangelical Biblicism. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
  78. Marcus J, Flannery KV (2004) The coevolution of ritual and society: New C-14 dates from ancient Mexico. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 101: 18257–18261.Google Scholar
  79. Marks L (2004) Sacred practices in highly religious families. Family Process 43: 217–231.Google Scholar
  80. Mathew S, Boyd R (2009) When does optional participation allow the evolution of cooperation? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 276: 1167–1174.Google Scholar
  81. Mazar N, Ariely D (2006) Dishonesty in everyday life and its policy implications. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing 25: 117–126.Google Scholar
  82. McElreath R, Boyd R, Richerson PJ (2003) Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology 44: 122–129.Google Scholar
  83. McNeil WH (1982) Pursuit of Power. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  84. McNeil WH (1991) The Rise of the West. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  85. Norenzayan A, Atran S, Faulkner J, Schaller M (2006) Memory and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science 30: 531–553.Google Scholar
  86. Norenzayan A, Hansen IG (2006) Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32: 174–187.Google Scholar
  87. Norenzayan A, Shariff AF (2008) The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science 322: 58–62.Google Scholar
  88. Norris P, Inglehart R (2004) Sacred and Secular. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  89. Nowak MA, Sigmund K (2005) Indirect reciprocity. Nature 437: 1291–1298.Google Scholar
  90. Panchanathan K, Boyd R (2003) A tale of two defectors: The importance of standing for the evolution of indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology 224: 115–126.Google Scholar
  91. Panchanathan K, Boyd R (2004) Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature 432: 499–502.Google Scholar
  92. Phillips A (1970) Ancient Israel’s Criminal Law: A New Approach to the Decalogue. New York: Schocken.Google Scholar
  93. Pyysiäinen I (2003) Buddhism, religion, and the concept of “God.” Numen—International Review for the History of Religions 50: 147–171.Google Scholar
  94. Rakoczy H, Wameken F, Tomasello M (2008) The sources of normativity: Young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology 44: 875–881.Google Scholar
  95. Rappaport RA (1999) Ritual and Religion in the Making of Humanity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  96. Richerson P, Boyd R (2005) Not by Genes Alone. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  97. Roes FL (1995) The size of societies, stratification, and belief in high gods supportive of human morality. Politics and the Life Sciences 14: 73–77.Google Scholar
  98. Roes FL, Raymond M (2003) Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior 24: 126–135.Google Scholar
  99. Ruffle BJ, Sosis R (2006) Cooperation and the in-group out-group bias: A field test on Israeli kibbutz members and city residents. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 60: 147–163.Google Scholar
  100. Shariff AF, Norenzayan A (2007) God is watching you. Psychological Science 18: 803–809.Google Scholar
  101. Smith C (1996) Disruptive Religion. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  102. Sosis R, Alcorta C (2003) Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 264–274.Google Scholar
  103. Sosis R, Bressler ER (2003) Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research 37: 211–239.Google Scholar
  104. Sosis R, Kress H, Boster J (2007) Scars for war. Evolution and Human Behavior 28: 234–247.Google Scholar
  105. Sosis R, Ruffle BJ (2003) Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology 44: 713–722.Google Scholar
  106. Sperber D (1996) Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  107. Sperber D, Premack D, Premack A, eds (1995) Causal Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  108. Stark R (1997) The Rise of Christianity. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  109. Sweeney M (2001) King Josiah of Judah. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  110. Tetlock P (2003) Thinking the unthinkable: Sacred values and taboo cognitions. Trends in Cognitive Science 7: 320–324.Google Scholar
  111. Trainor L, Trehub S (1992) The development of referential meaning in music. Music Perception 9: 455–470.Google Scholar
  112. Whitehouse H (2004) Modes of Religiosity: A Cognitive Theory of Religious Transmission. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
  113. Whitson JA, Galinsky AD (2008) Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science 322: 115–117.Google Scholar
  114. Whythe WF (1993) Street Corner Society: The Social Structure of an Italian Slum. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  115. Wilson DS (2002) Darwin’s Cathedral: Evolution, Religion, and the Nature of Society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  116. Wiltermuth SS, Heath C (2009) Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science 20: 1–5.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institut Jean NicodCNRSParisFrance
  2. 2.John Jay College of Criminal JusticeNew YorkUSA
  3. 3.ISR and Ford School of Public PolicyUniversity of MichiganAnn ArborUSA
  4. 4.Department of Economics and Department of PsychologyUniversity of British ColumbiaVancouverCanada

Personalised recommendations