Advertisement

Biology Bulletin

, Volume 44, Issue 4, pp 425–429 | Cite as

Effect of ambient odor on cognitive functions in children

  • E. I. RodionovaEmail author
  • A. V. Minor
Animal and Human Physiology
  • 83 Downloads

Abstract

We studied the effect of the peppermint ambient odor on the performance of routine Russian school language tests by primary-school students in a standard classroom, where a noiseless source of odor provided scenting the air by means of free evaporation of the essential oil at a constant low concentration. It is shown that in the presence of peppermint odor the marks for a word dictation test that is based on the longterm memory are significantly higher than in the absence of the odor, whereas the performance of text copying test (based mainly on attention) is not influenced by the presence of peppermint odor.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Androshchuk, A.A., Daletskaya, L.P., Lebeda, A.F., and Makarchuk, N.M., The use of aromatic volatile biologically active compounds of plant origin in infant schools for the prevention of respiratory diseases, Pediatr., 1986, no. 8, pp. 44–46.Google Scholar
  2. Clark, M., Mood and Helping: Mood as a Motivator of Helping and Helping as a Regulator of Mood, Newbury Park, California: Sage Publ. Inc., 1991.Google Scholar
  3. Doty, R.L., Shaman, P., Applebaum, S.L., Giberson, R., Siksorski, L., and Rosenberg, L., Smell identification ability: changes with age, Science, 1984, vol. 226, pp. 1441–1443.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Epple, G. and Herz, R.S., Ambient odors associated to failure influence cognitive performance in children, Develop. Psychobiol., 1999, vol. 35, pp. 103–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Goel, N. and Lau, R.P., Sleep changes vary by odor perception in young adults, Biol. Psychol., 2006, vol. 71, pp. 341–349.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Herz, R.S., Aromatherapy facts and fictions: a scientific analysis of olfactory effects on mood, physiology and behavior, Int. J. Neurosci., 2009, vol. 119, pp. 263–290.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Herz, R.S. and von Clef, J., The influence of verbal labeling on the perception of odors: evidence for olfactory illusions?, Perception, 2001, vol. 30, pp. 381–391.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Herz, R.S., Schankler, C., and Beland, S., Olfaction, emotion and associative learning: effects on motivated behavior, Motivat. Emotion, 2004, vol. 28, pp. 363–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ho, C. and Spence, C., Olfactory facilitation of dual-task performance, Neurosci. Lett., 2005, vol. 389, pp. 35–40.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Ilmberger, J., Heuberger, E., Mahrhoferm, C., Dessovic, H., Kowarik, D., and Buchbauer, G., The influence of essential oils on human attention. I: Alertness, Chem. Senses, 2001, vol. 26, pp. 239–245.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Knasko, S.C., Ambient odor’s effect on creativity, mood, and perceived health, Chem. Senses, 1992, vol. 17, pp. 27–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lorig, T.S. and Roberts, M., Odor and cognitive alternation of the contingent negative variation, Chem. Senses, 1990, vol. 14, pp. 525–536.Google Scholar
  13. Ludvigson, H.W. and Rottman, T.R., Effects of ambient odors of lavender and cloves on cognition, memory, affect and mood, Chem. Senses, 1989, vol. 14, pp. 525–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Macfarlane, A., Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate, Ciba Found Symp., 1975, vol. 33, pp. 103–117.Google Scholar
  15. Mallet, P. and Schaal, B., Rating and recognition of peers’ personal odors by 9-year-old children: an exploratory study, J. Gen. Psychol., 1998, vol. 125, pp. 47–64.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Michael, G.A., Jacquot, L., Millot, J.-L., and Brand, G., Ambient odors modulate visual attentional capture, Neurosci. Lett., 2003, vol. 352, pp. 221–225.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Moss, M., Cook, J., Wesnes, K., and Duckett, P., Aromas of rosemary and lavender essential oils differentially affect cognition and mood in healthy adults, Int. J. Neurosci., 2003, vol. 113, pp. 15–38.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Moss, M., Hewitt, S., Moss, L., and Wesnes, K., Modulation of cognitive performance and mood by aromas of peppermint and ylang-ylang, Int. J. Neurosci., 2008, vol. 118, pp. 59–77.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Richardson, J.T.E. and Zucco, G.M., Cognition and olfaction: a review, Psychol. Bull., 1989, vol. 105, pp. 352–360.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Schaal, B., Olfaction in infants and children: developmental and functional perspectives, Chem. Senses, 1988, vol. 13, pp. 145–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Schleidt, M., Hold, B., and Attili, G., A cross-cultural study on the attitude towards personal odors, J. Chem. Ecol., 1981, vol. 7, pp. 19–31.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Van Toller, S. and Dodd, G.H., Perfumery. The Psychology and Biology of Fragrance, New York: Chapman and Hall, 1988.Google Scholar
  23. Villemure, C., Slotnick, B.M., and Bushnell, M.C., Effects of odors on pain perception: deciphering the roles of emotion and attention, Pain, 2003, vol. 106, pp. 101–108.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Weisfeld, G.E., Czilli, T., Phillips, K.A., Gall, J.A., and Lichtman, C.M., Possible olfaction-based mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance, J. Exp. Child. Psychol., 2003, vol. 85, pp. 279–295.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Wright, T.A. and Staw, B.M., Affect and favorable work outcomes: two longitudinal tests of the happy-productive worker thesis, Organizat. Behav., 1999, vol. 20, pp. 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Pleiades Publishing, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Kharkevich Institute of Information Transmission ProblemsRussian Academy of SciencesMoscowRussia
  2. 2.Severtsov Institute of Problems of Ecology and EvolutionRussian Academy of SciencesMoscowRussia

Personalised recommendations