Skip to main content
Log in

The Effect of a 21-Day Dry Immersion on Ponzo and Müller–Lyer Illusions

  • Published:
Human Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the course of a 21-day Dry Immersion (DI), we studied the strength of Ponzo and Müller–Lyer visual illusions with the help of verbal response and two variants of the motor response by means of “tracking” (with the presence and absence of visual feedback). The studies were performed with the participation of ten subjects before the start of DI, on the 3rd, 10th, and 20th day of DI, as well as at the end of it. It was shown that DI mainly affects the motor response. Changes in reducing the strength of visual illusions were more pronounced for a more complex motor task performed in the absence of visual feedback. The results are in agreement with the data that we previously obtained under the conditions of a 5-day DI. One of the possible mechanisms of the observed changes in the illusions estimates may be hyperactivation of the dorsal visual pathway due to gravitational unloading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 4.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  1. Velichkovskii, B.M., Kognitivnaya nauka: Osnovy psikhologii poznaniya (Cognitive Science: Fundamentals of the Psychology of Cognition), in 2 vols., Moscow: Smysl, 2006, p. 162.

  2. Suzuki, Y., Fujii, K., and Onizawa, T., Studies on sensory deprivation: IV. Part 6: Effects of sensory deprivation upon perceptual function, Tohoku Psychol. Folia, 1965, vol. 24, p. 24.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Lybrand, W.A., Andrews, T.G., and Ross, S., Systemic fatigue and perceptual organization, Am. J. Psychol., 1954, vol. 67, no. 4, p. 704.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. King, J.P., Christensen, B.K., and Westwood, D.A., Grasping behavior in schizophrenia suggests selective impairment in the dorsal visual pathway, J. Abnorm. Psychol., 2008, vol. 117, no. 4, p. 799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. King, D.J., Hodgekins, J., Chouinard, P.A., et al., A review of abnormalities in the perception of visual illusions in schizophrenia, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 2017, vol. 24, no. 3, p. 734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Grzeczkowski, L., Roinishvili, M.O., Chkonia, E.D., et al., Is the perception of illusions abnormal in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Res., 2018, vol. 270, p. 929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Gori, S., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A., Visual illusions: an interesting tool to investigate developmental dyslexia and autism spectrum disorder, Front. Hum. Neurosci., 2016, vol. 10, p. 175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Giese, M.A. and Rizzolatti, G., Neural and computational mechanisms of action processing: Interaction between visual and motor representations, Neuron, 2015, vol. 88, no. 1, p. 167.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Dokka, K., DeAngelis, G.C., and Angelaki, D.E., Multisensory integration of visual and vestibular signals improves heading discrimination in the presence of a moving object, J. Neurosci., 2015, vol. 35, no. 40, p. 13599.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Grabherr, L. and Mast, F.W., Effects of microgravity on cognition: the case of mental imagery, J. Vestibular Res., 2010, vol. 20, no. 1, p. 53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Tomilovskaya, E., Shigueva, T., Sayenko, D., Rukavishnikov, I., and Kozlovskaya, I., Dry immersion as a ground-based model of microgravity physiological effects, Front. Physiol., 2019, vol. 10, p. 284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Sosnina, I.S., Lyakhovetskii, V.A., Zelenskiy, K.A., Karpinskaya, V.Yu., and Tomilovskaya, E.S., Effects of five-day “dry” immersion on the strength of the Ponzo and the Müller–Lyer illusions, Neurosci. Behav. Physiol., 2019, vol. 49, no. 2, p. 847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cheron, G., Leroy, A., Palmero-Soler, E., et al., Gravity influences top-down signals in visual processing, PLoS One, 2014, vol. 9, no. 1, p. e82371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Shoshina, I.I., Sosnina, I.S., Zelenskiy, K.A., et al., The functional state of the magnocellular and parvocellular neuronal systems in microgravity conditions, Materialy Pyatnadtsatogo mezhdunarodnogo mezhdistsiplinarnogo kongressa “Neironauka dlya meditsiny i psikhologii,” Sudak, 30 maya–10 iyunya 2019 g. (Proc. Fifteenth Int. Interdisciplinary Congr. “Neuroscience for Medicine and Psychology,” Sudak, May 30–June 10, 2019), Sudak, 2019, p. 474.

  15. Oldfield, R.C., The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory, Neuropsychologia, 1971, vol. 9, p. 97.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Aarts, E., Verhage, M., Veenvliet, J.V., et al., A solution to dependency: using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data, Nat. Neurosci., 2014, vol. 17, p. 491.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Chetverikov, A.A., Linear models with mixed effects in cognitive researches, Ross. Zh. Kognit. Nauki, 2015, vol. 2, no. 1, p. 41.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Karpinskaya, V. and Lyakhovetskii, V., Differences in sensorimotor assessment of Ponzo’s and Müller–Lyer’s illusions, Psikhol. Issled., 2014, vol. 7, no. 38, p. 3.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Martín-Loeches, M., Valdés, B., Gómez-Jarabo, G., and Rubia, F.J., Working memory within the visual dorsal stream: brain potentials of spatial location and motion direction encoding into memory, Int. J. Neurosci., 1998, vol. 96, nos. 1–2, p. 87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Searleman, A., Porac, C., Alvin, J., and Peaslee, K., Manipulating the strength of the Ponzo and horizontal–vertical illusions through extraction of local cue information, Am. J. Psychol., 2009, vol. 122, no. 3, p. 383.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Pechenkova, E., Nosikova, I., Rumshiskaya, A., et al., Alterations of functional brain connectivity after long-duration spaceflight as revealed by fMRI, Front. Physiol., 2019, vol. 10, p. 761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Dijkerman, H.C. and de Haan, E.H., Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action, Behav. Brain Sci., 2007, vol. 30, no. 2, p. 189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Husain, M. and Nachev, P., Space and the parietal cortex, Trends Cognit. Sci., 2007, vol. 1, no. 11, p. 30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Kornilova, L.N. and Tarasov, I.K., Orientation illusions in weightlessness, Aviakosm. Ekol. Med., 1996, vol. 30, no. 3, p. 17.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Kornilova, L.N., Glukhikh, D.O., Habarova, E.V., et al., Visual–manual tracking after long spaceflights, Hum. Physiol., 2016, vol. 42, no. 3, p. 301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Kornilova, L.N., Naumov, I.A., Mazurenko, A.Yu., and Kozlovskaya, I.B., Visual-manual tracking and vestibular function during a seven-day dry immersion, Hum. Physiol., 2010, vol. 36, no. 7, p. 813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Jang, S.H. and Jang, W.H., The different association of allocentric and egocentric neglect with dorsal and ventral pathways: a case report, Medicine (Baltimore), 2018, vol. 97, no. 37, p. e12394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Men’shikova, G.Ya., Constructive and ecological approaches to the study of the visual perception: analysis of differences, Vestn. Mosk. Univ., Ser. 14: Psikhol., 2007, vol. 4, p. 34.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Badakva, A.M., Miller, N.V., Zobova, L.N., and Roshchin, V.Yu., The influence of support unloading on the cortical control of hand movements in immersion experiments on monkeys, Aviakosm. Ekol. Med., 2019, vol. 53, no. 3, p. 33.

    Google Scholar 

  30. McIntyre, J. and Lipshits, M., Central processes amplify and transform anisotropies of the visual system in a test of visual–haptic coordination, J. Neurosci., 2008, vol. 28, no. 5, p. 1246.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The development of methods for assessing illusions and data analysis was carried out with the support of the Russian Foundation for Basic Research (project no. 19-013-00036). Organization of immersion studies and participation of E.S. Tomilovskaya were supported by the RAS project (63.1).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to I. S. Sosnina.

Ethics declarations

COMPLIANCE WITH ETHICAL STANDARDS

All studies were carried out in accordance with the principles of biomedical ethics formulated in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its subsequent updates, and approved by the local bioethical committee of the Institute for Biomedical Problems RAS (Moscow).

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare no obvious and potential conflicts of interest related to the publication of this article.

INFORMED CONSENT

Each the study participant submitted a voluntary written informed consent signed by him after explaining to him the potential risks and benefits, as well as the nature of the upcoming study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sosnina, I.S., Lyakhovetskii, V.A., Zelenskiy, K.A. et al. The Effect of a 21-Day Dry Immersion on Ponzo and Müller–Lyer Illusions. Hum Physiol 47, 51–59 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119721010138

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119721010138

Keywords:

Navigation