Skip to main content
Log in

Delayed motor production of open polygonal chains presented visually in static and dynamic modes: Comparison between 9- to 11-year-old children and adults

  • Published:
Human Physiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Production of drawing movements was studied in 29 right-handed children aged 9 to 11 years. The movements were sequences of horizontal and vertical linear strokes conjoined at a right angle (open polygonal chains) referred to below as trajectories. Trajectories comprising four to six segments were presented to a subject visually in a static or dynamic mode (i.e., as a linear drawing or as a moving cursor leaving no trace). The subjects were asked to draw (copy) the trajectory as fast as possible without lifting the pen in response to a delayed go-signal (short click). The parameters analyzed were the production latency time, the average duration of movement along a trajectory segment, and the overall number of errors made by a subject while producing the trajectory. Comparison of the data obtained in children with similar data for adults (16 subjects) revealed the following patterns. First, between the ages of 9 and 11 years, the error rate rapidly decreased for both the static and the dynamic modes of presentation, although 11-year-old children still made more errors than adults. Second, the average movement duration shortened with age, while the latency time tended to increase. Third, in 9- to 11-year-old children, in contrast to adults, the latency times for the static and dynamic modes of trajectory presentation did not differ from each other. The differences in trajectory production between adults and children are attributed to the predominant involvement of on-line programming in children and preprogramming in adults.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Bullock, D., Adaptive neural models of queuing and timing in fluent action, Trends Cogn. Sci., 2004, vol. 8, no. 9, p. 426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Bradley, C. and Pearson, J., The sensory components of high-capacity iconic memory and visual working memory, Front. Psychol., 2012, vol. 3, p. 355.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Rensink, R.A., Limits to the use of iconic memory, Perception, 2008, vol. 37, p. 45.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Sligte, I.G., Scholte, H.S., and Lamme, V.A., Are there multiple visual short-term memory stores?, PLoS One, 2008, vol. 3, no. 2, e1699.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bays, P.M. and Husain, M., Dynamic shifts of limited working memory resources in human vision, Science, 2008, vol. 321, no. 5890, p. 851.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Matsukura, M. and Hollingworth, A., Does visual short-term memory have a high-capacity stage?, Psychon. Bull. Rev., 2011, vol. 18, no. 6, p. 1098.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Korneev, A.A. and Kurgansky, A.V., Internal representation of movement sequences built upon either static line drawings or trajectories of a moving object, Zh. Vyssh. Nervn. Deyat. im. I.P. Pavlova, 2013, vol. 63, no. 4, p. 437.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Davidson, M.C., Amso, D., Anderson, L.C., and Diamond, A., Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching, Neuropsychologia, 2006, vol. 44, no. 11, p. 2037.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Luciana, M., Conklin, H.M., Hooper, C.J., and Yarger, R.S., The development of nonverbal working memory and executive control processes in adolescents, Child. Dev., 2005, vol. 76, no. 3, p. 697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Kurgansky, A.V., Development and regulation of finely coordinated cyclic hand movements in children of preschool and early school age, Novye Issled., 2013, vol. 40, no. 1. p. 9.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Dunabeitia, J.A., Dimitropoulou, M., Estevez, A., and Carreiras, M., The influence of reading expertise in mirror-letter perception: evidence from beginning and expert readers, Mind Brain Educ., 2013, vol. 7, no. 2. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12017

    Google Scholar 

  12. Schott, G.D., Mirror writing: neurological reflections on an unusual phenomenon, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry, 2007, vol. 78, no. 1, p. 5.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kurgansky, A.V., The relationship between the maximal rate of production of simple periodic hand movement sequences and their structural complexity. The age factor, Novye Issled., 2011, vol. 28, no. 3, p. 18.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Kurgansky, A.V. and Grigal, P.P., Directed corticocortical functional connectivity at the early stages of serial learning in adults and seven- to eight-year old children, Hum. Physiol., 2010, vol. 36, no. 4, p. 408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Yan, J.H., Thomas, J.R., Stelmach, G.E., and Thomas, K.T., Developmental features of rapid aiming arm movements across the lifespan, J. Mot. Behav., 2000, vol. 32, no. 2, p. 121.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kurgansky, A.V. and Kurgansky, M.E., Age-related changes in the spatiotemporal structure of simple cyclical graphic movements performed at the maximal rate: I. The increase in the movement rate is accounted for by reduction of the number of submovements in a movement cycle, Hum. Physiol., 2011, vol. 37, no. 1, p. 22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Chartrel, E. and Vinter, A., The impact of spatio-temporal constraints on cursive letter handwriting in children, Learn. Instr., 2008, vol. 18, p. 537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kiselev, S., Espy, K.A., and Sheffield, T., Age-related differences in reaction time task performance in young children, J. Exp. Child Psychol., 2009, vol. 102, no. 2, p. 150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Badan, M., Hauert, C.A., and Mounoud, P., Sequential pointing in children and adults, J. Exp. Child Psychol., 2000, vol. 75, no. 1, p. 43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ketelaars, A.C., Garry, M.I., and Franks, I.M., Online programming of simple movement sequences, Hum. Mov. Sci., 1997, vol. 16, p. 461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Smiley-Oyen, A.L. and Worringham, C.J., Peripheral constraint versus on-line programming in rapid aimed sequential movements, Acta Psychol. (Amst.), 2001, vol. 108, no. 3, p. 219.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Rhodes, B.J., Bullock, D., Verwey, W.B., Averbeck, B.B., and Page, M.P., Learning and production of movement sequences: behavioral, neurophysiological, and modeling perspectives, Hum. Mov. Sci., 2004, vol. 23, no. 5, p. 699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to A. V. Kurgansky.

Additional information

Original Russian Text © A.A. Antonova, K.A. Absatova, A.A. Korneev, A.V. Kurgansky, 2015, published in Fiziologiya Cheloveka, 2015, Vol. 41, No. 2, pp. 38–45.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Antonova, A.A., Absatova, K.A., Korneev, A.A. et al. Delayed motor production of open polygonal chains presented visually in static and dynamic modes: Comparison between 9- to 11-year-old children and adults. Hum Physiol 41, 143–149 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119715020036

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1134/S0362119715020036

Keywords

Navigation