Advertisement

Russian Journal of Marine Biology

, Volume 42, Issue 2, pp 196–198 | Cite as

Asymmetry of pectoral flipper use in the orca Orcinus orca (Linnaeus, 1758) from the Avachinskii Bay (Eastern Kamchatka)

  • A. N. Giljov
  • K. A. Karenina
  • T. V. Ivkovich
  • Y. B. Malashichev
Ethology

Abstract

Asymmetrical forelimb use is characteristic of many mammalian species. However, little is known about this phenomenon in cetaceans. We examined the asymmetry in pectoral flipper use by the wild orca Orcinus orca (Linnaeus, 1758). During observations from motor boats in the Avachinskii Bay (East Kamchatka) we recorded the use of flippers in slapping the water by individually identified resident (fish-eating) orcas. The studied individuals mostly used their right flipper. The revealed right-sided bias presumably reflects the pivotal role of the left brain hemisphere in the control of forelimb movements, which is consistent with the data that have been obtained for other cetacean species.

Keywords

orca motor asymmetry pectoral flipper brain asymmetry lateralization cetaceans 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Karenina, K.A., Giljov, A.N., Malashichev, Y.B., et al., Visual lateralization in wild: perceiving of novel object in beluga whale (Delphinapterus leucas), J. Asymmetry, 2010, vol. 4, no. 2, pp. 3–12.Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Nagailik, M.M., Filatova, O.A., Ivkovich T.V., et al., Area usage by killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Avacha Gulf of Kamchatka., Zool. Zh., 2010, vol. 89, no. 4, pp. 484–494.Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Filatova, O.A., Ivkovich, T.V., Shpak, O.V., et al., Killer whales—fishers and hunters, Priroda (Moscow, Russ. Fed.), 2013, no 5, pp. 28–37.Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Clapham, P.J., Leimkuhler, E., and Gray, B.K., Do humpback whales exhibit lateralized behaviour? Anim. Behav., 1995, vol. 50, pp. 73–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Clark, C.W., Acoustic behavior of mysticete whales, in Sensory Abilities of Cetaceans, Thomas, J.A. and Kastelein, R.A., Eds., New York: Springer, 1990), pp. 571–583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Dunlop, R.A., Cato, D.H., and Noad, M.J., Non-song acoustic communication in migrating humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae), Mar. Mamm. Sci., 2008, vol. 24, pp. 613–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Felix, F., Bearson, B., and Falconi, J., Epizoic barnacles removed from the skin of a humpback whale after a period of intense surface activity, Mar. Mamm. Sci., 2006, vol. 22, pp. 979–984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Giljov, A., Karenina, K., Ingram, J., and Malashichev, Y., Parallel emergence of true handedness in the evolution of marsupials and placentals, Curr. Biol., 2015, vol. 25, no. 14, pp. 1878–1884.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Hopkins, W.D. and Nir, T.M., Planum temporale surface area and grey matter asymmetries in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the effect of handedness and comparison with findings in humans, Behav. Brain Res., 2010, vol. 208, pp. 436–443.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Hopkins, W.D., Phillips, K.A., Bania, A., et al., Hand preferences for coordinated bimanual actions in 777 great apes: implications for the evolution of handedness in hominins, J. Hum. Evol., 2011, vol. 60, pp. 605–611.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Ivkovich, T., Filatova, O.A., Burdin, A.M., et al., The social organization of resident-type killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Avacha Gulf, Northwest Pacific, as revealed through association patterns and acoustic similarity, Mamm. Biol., 2010, vol. 75, pp. 198–210.Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Johnson, C.M. and Moewe, K., Pectoral fin preference during contact in Commerson’s dolphins (Cephalorhynchus commersonii), Aquat. Mamm., 1999, vol. 25, pp. 73–77.Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Karenina, K., Giljov, A., Ivkovich, T., et al., Lateralization of spatial relationships between wild mother and infant orcas, Orcinus orca, Anim. Behav., 2013, vol. 86, no. 6, pp. 1225–1231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Karenina K., Giljov A., Ivkovich T., and Malashichev Y., Evidence for the perceptual origin of right-sided feeding biases in cetaceans, Anim. Cogn., 2016, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 239–243. doi 10.1007/s10071-015-0899-4CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    MacNeilage, P.F., Vertebrate whole-body-action asymmetries and the evolution of right handedness: a comparison between humans and marine mammals, Dev. Psychobiol., 2013, vol. 55, no. 6, pp. 577–587.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Raymond, M. and Pontier, D., Is there geographical variation in human handedness? Laterality, 2004, vol. 9, pp. 35–52.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Rogers, L.J., Hand and paw preferences in relation to the lateralized brain, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B., 2009, vol. 364, pp. 943–954.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Rosa-Salva, O., Regolin, L., Mascalzoni, E., and Vallortigara, G., Cerebral and behavioural asymmetries in animal social recognition, Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev., 2012, vol. 7, pp. 110–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Sakai, M., Hishii, T., Takeda, S., et al., Laterality of flipper rubbing behaviour in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus): caused by asymmetry of eye use?, Behav. Brain Res., 2006, vol. 170, pp. 204–210.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Ströckens, F., Güntürkün, O., and Ocklenburg, S., Limb preferences in non-human vertebrates, Laterality, 2013, vol. 18, no. 5, pp. 536–575.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Wahlberg, M., Lunneryd, S.-G., and Westerberg, H., The source level of harbour seal flipper slaps, Aquat. Mamm., 2002, vol. 28, no. 1, pp. 90–92.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Pleiades Publishing, Ltd. 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  • A. N. Giljov
    • 1
  • K. A. Karenina
    • 1
  • T. V. Ivkovich
    • 1
  • Y. B. Malashichev
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.St. Petersburg State UniversitySt. PetersburgRussia
  2. 2.Institute of Experimental MedicineSt. PetersburgRussia

Personalised recommendations