Advertisement

Social Theory & Health

, Volume 17, Issue 2, pp 140–157 | Cite as

Mental health, subjectivities and forms of neuroscience: a critical realist examination

  • David PilgrimEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

The examination of personal experience in human science has been highly variegated. At one end of a spectrum, strong subjectivists prioritise and privilege personal experience as an authentic marker of being human and as a window into our embedding social contexts. At the other end are neuro-reductionists, who explain (or even explain away) personal experience as merely an epiphenomenon of brain activity. With a focus on mental health and psychiatry, critical realism is used to explore this spectrum and it endorses a view that the brain affords our capacity to think, feel and act as human agents in contingent contexts but cannot ultimately explain any of these.

Keywords

Neuroscience Subjectivities Critical realism Biopsychosocial model 

Notes

References

  1. Anda, R., B. Felliti, J.D. Bremner, J. Walker, C. Whitfield, and B. Perry. 2006. The Enduring Effects of Abuse and Related Adverse Experiences in Childhood: A Convergence of Evidence from Neurobiology and Epidemiology. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 265 (3): 174–186.Google Scholar
  2. Angell, M. 2011. The Illusions of Psychiatry. Review of Books. New York.Google Scholar
  3. Archer, M. 1997. Realist Social Science: The Morphogenetic Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Archer, M. 2000. Being Human: The Problem of Agency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Benton, T. 1991. Biology and Social Science: Why the Return of the Repressed Should be Given a (Cautious) Welcome. Sociology 25 (1): 63–68.Google Scholar
  6. Bhaskar, R. 2016. Enlightened Common Sense: The Philosophy of Critical Realism. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  7. Bhaskar, R., and B. Danermark. 2006. Metatheory, Interdisciplinarity and Disability Research: A Critical Realist Perspective. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research 4: 278–297.Google Scholar
  8. Breggin, P., and G. Breggin. 1994. Talking Back to Prozac. New York: St Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  9. Broad, C.D. 1925. The Mind and Its Place in Nature. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  10. Campbell, P., and D. Rose. 2011. Action for Change in the UK: Thirty Years of The user/Survivor Movement. In The SAGE Handbook of Mental Health and Illness, ed. D. Pilgrim, A. Rogers, and B. Pescosolido. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  11. Clayton, P., and P. Davies. 2006. The Re-emergence of Emergence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Cutajar, M.C., P.E. Mullen, J.R.P. Ogloff, S.D. Thomas, D.L. Wells, and J. Spataro. 2010. Psychopathology in a Large Cohort of Sexually Abused Children Followed up to 43 Years. Child Abuse and Neglect 34 (11): 813–822.Google Scholar
  13. De Groot, M., S.H. Golden, and J. Wagner. 2016. Psychological Conditions in Adults with Diabetes. American Psychologist 71: 552.Google Scholar
  14. Dennett, D. 1984. Elbow Room. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  15. Engel, G. 1977. The Need for a New Medical Model: A Challenge for Biomedicine. Science 196 (4286): 129–136.Google Scholar
  16. Engel, G.L. 1980. The Clinical Application of the Biopsychosocial Model. American Journal of Psychiatry 137: 535–544.Google Scholar
  17. Erickson, K., C. Hillman, and A. Kramer. 2015. Physical Activity, Brain, and Cognition. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 4: 27–32.Google Scholar
  18. Fitzgerald, D., N. Rose, and I. Singh. 2016. Revitalizing Sociology: Urban Life and Mental Illness Between History and the Present. British Journal of Sociology 67 (1): 138–160.Google Scholar
  19. Flatschart, E. 2017. Feminist Standpoints and Critical Realism: The Contested Materiality of Difference in Intersectionality and New Materialism. Journal of Critical Realism 16 (3): 284–302.Google Scholar
  20. Frances, A. (2010) Opening Pandora’s Box: The 19 worst suggestions for DSM-5. Psychiatric Times.Google Scholar
  21. Fullagar, S. 2009. Negotiating the Neurochemical Self: Anti-depressant Consumption in Women’s Recovery from Depression. Health 13: 389–406.Google Scholar
  22. Galea, S., M. Uddin, and K. Koenen. 2011. The Urban Environment and Mental Disorders: Epigenetic Links’. Epigenetics 6 (4): 400–404.Google Scholar
  23. Garcia-Toro, M., and I. Auirre. 2007. Biopsychosocial Model in Depression. Medical Hypotheses 68 (3): 683–691.Google Scholar
  24. Guze, S.B. 1989. Biological Psychiatry: Is There Any Other Kind? Psychological Medicine 19: 315–323.Google Scholar
  25. Harré, R. 2002. Cognitive Science: A Philosophical Introduction. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  26. HBP-PS Consortium. 2012. The Human Brain Project: A Report to the European Commission. Lausanne: The HBP-PS Consortium.Google Scholar
  27. Healy, D. 2004. Psychopathology at the Interface Between the Market and the New Biology. In The New Brain Sciences: Perils and Prospects, ed. D. Rees and S. Rose. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Herzberg, D. 2009. Happy Pills in America: From Miltown to Prozac. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Higgs, P., J.R. Jones, and G. Scambler. 2004. Class as Variable, Class as Generative Mechanism: The Importance of Critical Realism for the Sociology of Health Inequalities. In Making Realism Work: Realist Theory and Empirical Research, ed. B. Carter and C. New, 52–63. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  30. Illlich, I. 1977. Medical Nemesis. London: Marion Boyars.Google Scholar
  31. Johnstone, L., and L. Dallos. 2006. Formulation in Psychology and Psychotherapy: Making Sense of People’s Problems. London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  32. Jones, E.G., and L.M. Mendell. 1999. Assessing the ‘Decade of the Brain‘. Science 284 (5415): 739.Google Scholar
  33. Kingdon, D., and A.H. Young. 2007. Research into Putative Biological Mechanisms of Mental Disorders has been of No Value to Clinical Psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry 191 (4): 285–290.Google Scholar
  34. Kirsch, I. 2009. The Emperor’s New Drugs: Exploding the Antidepressant Myth. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  35. Lakoff, A. 2005. Pharmaceutical Reason: Knowledge and Values in Global Psychiatry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Landecker, H., and A. Panofsky. 2013. From Social Structure to Gene Regulation, and back: A Critical Introduction to enviRonmental Epigenetics for Sociology. Annual Review of Sociology 39 (1): 333–357.Google Scholar
  37. Lederbogen, F., P. Kirsch, L. Haddad, et al. 2011. City Living and Urban Upbringing Affect Neural Social Stress Processing in Humans. Nature 474: 498–501.Google Scholar
  38. Lemert, C. 1980. Sociology and the Twilight of Man. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Margolis, J. 1990. Reconciling Realism and Relativism. In The Rhetorical Turn: Invention and Persuasion in the Conduct of Inquiry, ed. H. Simons. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Meyer, A. 1952. The Collected Works of Adolf Meyer. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  41. Mingers, J. 2011. The Contribution of Systemic Thought to Critical Realism. Journal of Critical Realism 10 (3): 303–330.Google Scholar
  42. Moncrieff, J. 2008. The Myth of the Chemical Cure. Basingstoke: Palgrave.Google Scholar
  43. Pilgrim, D. 2007. The Survival of Psychiatric Diagnosis. Social Science and Medicine 65 (3): 536–544.Google Scholar
  44. Pilgrim, D. 2008. The Eugenic Legacy in Psychology and Psychiatry. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 54 (3): 272–284.Google Scholar
  45. Pilgrim, D. 2015a. Understanding Mental Health: A Critical Realist Exploration. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  46. Pilgrim, D. 2015b. The Biopsychosocial Model in Health Research: Its’ Strength and Limitations for Critical Realists. Journal of Critical Realism 14 (2): 164–180.Google Scholar
  47. Pitt-Taylor, V. 2016. The Brain’s Body: Neuroscience and Corporeal Politics. London: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Read, J. 2005. The Bio-Bio-Bio Model of Madness. The Psychologist 18 (40): 596–597.Google Scholar
  49. Read, J., R. Fosse, A. Moskowitz, and B. Perry. 2014. The Traumagenic Neurodevelopmental Model of Psychosis Revisited. Neuropsychiatry 4 (1): 64–79.Google Scholar
  50. Rose, N. 1990. Governing the Soul. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  51. Rose, N. 2013. The Human Sciences in a Biological Age. Theory, Culture and Society 30 (1): 3–34.Google Scholar
  52. Rose, D. 2017. Service User/Survivor-Led Research in Mental Health: Epistemological Possibilities. Disability and Society 32 (6): 773–789.Google Scholar
  53. Rose, S., and H. Rose. 2012. Genes, Cells and Brains: Bioscience’s Promethean Promises. London: Verso.Google Scholar
  54. Rosenfield, S. 2012. Triple jeopardy? Mental Health at the Intersection of Gender, Race, and Class. Social Science and Medicine 74 (1): 1791–1796.Google Scholar
  55. Sarewitz, D., and R. Nelson. 2008. Three Rules for Technological Fixes. Nature 456: 871–872.Google Scholar
  56. Schilling, E.A., R.H. Aseltine, and S. Gore. 2008. The Impact of Cumulative Childhood Adversity on Young Adult Mental Health: Measures, Models, and Interpretations. Social Science and Medicine 6 (5): 1140–1151.Google Scholar
  57. Sedgwick, P. 1982. Psychopolitics. London: Pluto Press.Google Scholar
  58. Simons, H. 1990. The Rhetorical Turn: Invention and Persuasion in the Conduct of Inquiry. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Skinner, B.F. 1971. Beyond Freedom and Dignity. New York: Alfred A. Knopf Inc.Google Scholar
  60. Tallis, R. 2011. Aping Mankind: Neuromania, Darwinitis and the Misrepresentation of Humanity. London: Acumen.Google Scholar
  61. Tortorello, F. 2017. What is Real About Reductive Neuroscience? Journal of Critical Realism 16 (3): 235–254.Google Scholar
  62. Varela, C.R. 2003. Biological Structure and Embodied Human Agency: The Problem of Instinctivism. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 33 (1): 95–122.Google Scholar
  63. Whitaker, R. 2010. Anatomy of an Epidemic. New York: Broadway Press.Google Scholar
  64. Williams, S. 1999. Is Anybody There? Critical Realism, Chronic Illness and the Disability Debate. Sociology of Health and Illness 21: 797–819.Google Scholar
  65. Wittchen, H., F. Jacobi, et al. 2011. The Size and Burden of Mental Disorders and Other Disorders of the Brain in Europe 2010. European Neuropsychopharmacology 21 (9): 655–679.Google Scholar
  66. Wolman, B.B. 1981. Contemporary Theories and Systems in Psychology. Amsterdam: Plenum.Google Scholar
  67. Wykes, T., and F. Callard. 2010. Diagnosis, Diagnosis, Diagnosis: Towards DSM-5. Journal of Mental Health 19 (4): 301–304.Google Scholar
  68. Yates, A. 1970. Behavior Therapy. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Limited 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of LiverpoolLiverpoolUK
  2. 2.University of SouthamptonSouthamptonUK

Personalised recommendations