Abstract
Collective movements are essential for the effective function of animal societies, but are complicated by the need for consensus among group members. Consensus is typically assumed to arise via feedback mechanisms, but this ignores inter-individual variation in behavioural tendency (‘personality’), which is known to underpin the successful function of many complex societies. In this study, we use a theoretical approach to examine the relative importance of personality and feedback in the emergence of collective movement decisions in animal groups. Our results show that variation in personality dramatically influences collective decisions and can partially or completely replace feedback depending on the directionality of relationships among individuals. The influence of personality increases with the exaggeration of differences among individuals. While it is likely that both feedback and personality interact in nature, our findings highlight the potential importance of personality in driving collective processes.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Decision making is a frequent and fitness-critical behaviour in most animals. In group-living organisms this is complicated by a potential conflict of interests among group members, which nonetheless must collectively reach some form of consensus if the group is to act effectively while remaining together1. A combination of empirical and modelling studies has demonstrated that these complex collective actions observed in many group-living organisms can be explained by individuals following simple local rules coupled with mechanisms of feedback2,3,4,5,6,7,8. The broad success of feedback-based models in explaining the dynamics of collective actions in a wide range of taxa has led some to suggest that mechanisms such as this may be common to a wide range of group-living organisms from social insects to primates9,10.
In seeking to explain the complexity of animal collective behaviour, modelling approaches have often drawn from particle physics4, and focused on the influence of interactions while assuming that group members are identical entities. However, inter-individual variation is a central tenet of natural selection11, and it has long been recognised that variation in behavioural propensity among individuals underpins the successful function of complex societies such as those of social insects12,13. Furthermore, the recent abundance of studies in the field of ‘behavioural syndromes’ or ‘animal personality’ highlights the importance of this variability14,15,16,17,18. Behavioural variability among group members can be driven by differences in age, sex, condition or dominance status, among other things19,20,21. While not without controversy (e.g.,22), we use the term ‘personality’ here for reasons of word economy, to describe consistent individual variation in behaviour between individuals in a group, regardless of underlying cause of the variability. The available data indicate that the distribution of personality in a group can influence the outcome of various collective actions18,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31. For example, colonies of honeybees containing behaviourally more diverse individuals are collectively more successful foragers than less diverse colonies13 and groups with different mixed composition of shy/bold or social/asocial animals perform better than homogeneous groups in various taxa31,32,33. Personality differences can also lead to differences in how individuals contribute to decision making. For example, personality differences can give rise to leader–follower patterns in pairs of sticklebacks with bold fish emerging as leaders and shy fish emerging as followers in experimental pairs34. Furthermore, these differences can be enhanced by social feedback: bold leaders may inspire faithful followership, and shy followers facilitate effective leadership28,34. It is therefore likely that personality differences within groups interact with feedback mechanisms to influence the outcome of collective decisions.
One context in which collective decision making is crucial is group movements. These actions are essential for groups to track resources or evade threats while maintaining group cohesion35. Collective movement decisions are thought to arise through mechanisms of positive feedback based on active signals9,36,37,38. For example, whooper swans (Cygnus cygnus) increase honking frequency until a threshold (quorum) number of individuals are calling before the flock takes flight39. Swarms of honeybees are stimulated into movement by the piping sounds of scouts who have already reached a consensus on a suitable site to relocate to40. Movement decisions may also be cue-based, for example, if the departure of one individual from a group itself indicates a desire to initiate a collective movement41. This latter mechanism follows a basic sequence of events: one individual initiates a movement and the rest of the group may or may not follow this initiative. The effectiveness of the initiative then depends on the propensity of other individuals to follow, and that of the initiator to persist in the action. Thus, the initial movement will become a collective movement only if a number of other individuals follow the initiator42. These movements are characterised by an all-or-nothing, U-shaped, pattern of the number of responders following the initiator10,42,43,44. Thus, initiations that are not joined are common but unsuccessful, those joined by many individuals are common and successful, while intermediate numbers of joiners are rare (see Fig. 1). This kind of decision process has been observed in fish45,46, birds47 and mammals, including primates9,30,38,48, cattle49,50 and Prezwalski horses51.
Using a combination of modelling and natural observations, researchers have shown that these collective movements could be generated using two complementary feedback mechanisms, echoing the process of collective decision making in social insects10,38,52. The first of these is a mimetic process in which the propensity to follow depends on the number of individuals already following. The second is that the propensity of the initiator to give up is negatively associated with the number of followers10,50. These mechanisms are consistent with patterns observed in many species with shared decision processes10,38,53. However, most of these previous studies have excluded the potential effects of variability among individuals. Recent models of self-organised processes indicate that personality variation among group members27,43 and social affiliation between individuals53,54,55 are important for explaining the emergence of collective patterns. For instance, several authors have shown that, against theoretical expectations, some individuals can be more successful initiators42,50,53 or more inclined to follow52,54 than others during collective decisions. The degree to which these differences in behavioural tendencies may influence group decision making and the potential interaction between feedback mechanisms and personality variation remains largely unexplored.
In this study, we use a theoretical approach to examine the potential importance of personality variation in collective decision dynamics by modelling movement decisions in small animal groups. Specifically, we explore the relative importance of inter-individual variation in personality and different feedback mechanisms on the effectiveness of collective movements, thorough exploring the following themes: (i) how the distribution of personalities within the group influences decision making, (ii) whether personality effects can operate in a top-down (despotic) and/or bottom-up (democratic) manner and (iii) the influence that social feedback between initiator and followers play in decision making. We test the hypothesis that within-group variation in personality alone is sufficient for collective decisions to emerge, and can reproduce the U-shape response pattern characteristic of collective decisions in various organisms. We also assess the veracity of our model by comparison of results with data from a field study of collective movements in Capuchin monkeys.
Results
Simulation model development
Outline of model
We use an agent-based model to investigate the decision-making process during collective movements. We initially focus on small animal societies, as these are common to a wide range of taxa and simulation results can be compared with available field data. Specifically, we model the process of collective movements of groups of ten agents by adapting a model that Petit et al.10 used to simulate collective movements in Capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). We first outline their model and subsequently explain how this was modified to incorporate personality effects. Petit et al.10 considered that collective movement decisions arise through two mechanisms of feedback. The first one of these affects the probability of joining (PJ) a given initiation, while the second affects probability of cancelling (PC) this initiation. Specifically, PJ is a function of the total number of individuals in the group (N) and the number of individuals already departed (R):
Petit et al.10 used constants α = 162.3 and β = 75.4, which were inferred from field observations, that reflect the mimetic behaviour among individuals. Thus, the probability of joining, PJ, increases with the number of individuals already departed, while the probability of the initiator cancelling, PC, depends on the following equation:
where the PC of the initiator depends on a constant cancellation rate (δ) and decreases with the increasing numbers of responders (R). The constant e can be understood as an amplification effect depending on the influence of the individual who has joined the movement and γ is the threshold. Petit et al.10 obtained the best fit to observed data using values of δ = 0.009, γ = 2, and e = 2.3. If γ and e = 1, there is no amplification effect and thus any individual joining a movement will have the same effect on the initiator’s PC (see below).
Following the same premises, the general flow of our model is as follows: first, at t = 0, a random individual (the initiator) advertises its desire to move by departing the immediate vicinity of the group. We consider only a single initiator per movement for simplicity, though field observations of monkeys suggest that simultaneous initiations by multiple individuals are rare10,42,56. This corresponds to situations where the initiation probability is small compared to PJ and thus the time between initiations is greater than the duration of a collective movement. At subsequent time steps, the initiator has a probability of cancelling the movement PC, while the remaining individuals (referred to as responders or followers) decide whether or not to join the movement with a probability PJ (see Fig. 1).
Simulations were run for a period of 15 min with a timestep of one second. At each timestep, we updated individual behaviour based on the PC of the initiator and PJ of followers. Simulations were halted before the 15-min time limit if all responders joined the movement or if the initiator cancelled the movement. Following each initiation, we scored (i) whether or not the movement was successful (some or all individuals joined the initiator) or failed (initiator cancelled the movement) and (ii) how many individuals had joined the initiator at the time of cancellation (or at the end-time). While we used groups of ten individuals, the same process can be extended to groups comprising any number of individuals (see analytical model section).
Integration of personality
To integrate personality variation into movement dynamics, we remove the feedback function in Eq. 1 so that it becomes dependent on a single parameter α′:
where the PJ of a given responder r within a group of N individuals depends on a single parameter α′. This can be understood as a parameter defining the magnitude of a personality trait27,43,53, which influences collective movement decisions by altering the individual probability of joining, with higher values associated with lower joining probability. In our model, the parameter α′ has no effect on the probability of initiating a movement. All individuals thus have the same probability of becoming an initiator, which aligns with the available empirical data10,38,39,49,50,51.
Despotic and democratic scenarios
To understand how the distribution of traits might influence collective processes, it is necessary to elucidate how collective phenomena emerge from individual actions under different leadership scenarios57. We consider here that collective decisions can arise in a top-down (despotic) or bottom-up (democratic) manner. In despotic decisions, one or a few individuals assume the role of the leader and decide(s) for the rest of the group36, while democratic decisions typically emerge in a self-organised manner6,7,58,59. In the context of our model, the parameter α′ could determine both an individual’s social influence (its effect as an initiator on the propensity for others to follow) and the individual’s response threshold (its propensity to follow others).
From a top-down (despotic) perspective, the parameter α′ of the initiator determines the PJ of other group members and Eq. 3a becomes:
where the PJ of a responder r depends on the α′ of initiator i. Thus, when highly influential individuals act as initiators, they will have a strong influence on others (other individuals have a high and identical PJ), generating a rapid response from other group members, and resulting in a high probability of initiating a successful movement (making them effective leaders). On the other hand, low influence initiators will have more difficulty in attracting other individuals, and a low probability of initiating a successful movement (making them poor leaders).
Under the democratic scenario, the joining propensity PJ for each individual depends on its own α′, and is not influenced by the identity of the initiator. Individuals with low α′ (high PJ) will readily join movements regardless of the identity of the initiator, while individuals with high α′ have a low probability of joining movements. Equation 3a, thus becomes:
where the PJ of a responder r depends on its own α′, no matter who initiates the movement. This situation corresponds to some individuals being more eager to follow movements than others, as observed in52.
Modelling different distributions of personality
Animal personality can be considered a binary character state or a continuum depending on the trait in question, but presently there is little understanding of how such characters might be distributed within groups60,61. The type of distribution (e.g., uniform, normal or bimodal) may have strong implications for the emergence of collective behaviours such as decision making62,63,64,65.
To model the influence of the distribution of personalities in the group, we thus divided our simulations into two conditions. In the first, which we refer to as the unimodal condition (Uni), values of α′ were drawn from a truncated normal distribution. In the second, which we refer to as the bimodal condition (Bim), values of α′ are drawn from two separate truncated normal distributions, one of which has a higher mean than the other. Thus, in the unimodal condition, individual personalities are randomly distributed over a continuum of values, representing variability within a single class of individuals. This is in line with studies showing some individuals recruit followers faster or have more social bonds than others50. In the bimodal condition, groups are comprised of individuals with high or low α′, which is more representative of societies with distinct bold/shy or dominant/subordinate classes of individual66.
To test how the degree of inter-individual variability affected movement decisions, we proceeded as follows: in the case of Uni, we gradually expanded the range of the distribution of α′ from an initial value of 160 (in accordance with10) by values of 100 in both directions with maximum and minimum values of 10 and 4460. In the Bim scenario, the standard deviation of both normal distributions was fixed at 100 and the mean for low α′ individuals remained at 160, while the mean value of the high α′ distribution was increased by values of 100, to a maximum 3960. In all cases, we start with a set of simulations in which all individuals have the same value of α′, representing a control state without personality for each scenario. In our Bim scenarios we also modelled the influence of different leadership structures through modifying the proportion of high:low α′ individuals, by varying the number of individuals selected from each of the distributions (1:9 … 9:1).
Initiator’s propensity to cancel
Previous studies have indicated that the cancellation rate of the initiator (PC) is crucial for effective emulation of natural group patterns10,38,53. We consider four mechanisms that could underlie the decision of an initiator to cancel a movement. The first, as used in10, is Eq. 2 described above, where each responder influences the PC of the initiator in a non-linear way. We refer to this as the non-linear PC condition. The second mechanism (linear PC condition; Eq. 4) supposes that the addition of followers (R) influences the cancellation rate (C) of the leader in a linear manner instead of a quorum-like response like in Eq. 2, and this influence is proportional to the group size (N):
When considering a linear PC, we replace the cancellation rate δ with C (=0.004). As we do not consider any kind of feedback, C needs to be lower than δ, and this value was obtained by computing the mean value of Eq. 1 for N = 1…5. Note that the results obtained from Eq. 4 are qualitatively the same as those obtained from Eq. 2 with γ = 1 and e = 1.
Under the third mechanism (constant PC; Eq. 5):
individuals have a fixed cancellation rate C63, identical for all individuals and determined by their internal motivation state, which is independent of the number of followers or size of the group. Finally, we consider that the PC of initiator i is related to the personality of individual i, that is, proportional to its α′:
Thus, we hypothesise that individuals with lower α′ will have a lower rate of cancelling. We refer to this as the Cα′ condition. In these last two cases (Eqs. 5 and 6), we exclude any positive feedback between responders and initiator.
Simulations overview
The above scenarios are modelled in a full-factorial manner, the principal components of which are summarised in Fig. 2. Thus, we consider two cases where the PC of the initiator is under the influence of positive social feedback (non-linear and linear cancellation type), and two cases where there is no social effect (constant and Cα′). The control simulations (without personality) for each scenario were performed with the corresponding PC while maintaining the same α′ value (PJ) for all individuals. The control scenario for the non-linear condition, for instance, therefore corresponds to simulations with Eq. 3 (α′ = 160) and Eq. 2, while for constant and Cα′ conditions we used the most basic assumption: Eq. 2 (α′ = 160) and Eq. 5. These four cancellation types were modelled for each expanding range of α′, distributed either unimodally (Uni) or bimodally (Bim), and from a despotic (top-down) and democratic (bottom-up) perspective.
Assessing model performance
We use two approaches to assess the efficacy of each scenario and parameter combination in replicating movement patterns typical of natural groups. First, we assess the bimodality of the distribution of the frequency of different numbers of responding individuals for each combination of parameter values (see Methods for details). Bimodality of the response distribution can be considered indicative of an all-or-nothing type response typical of collective decisions in various organisms10,44,67. With respect to our model, bimodal response distributions are expected for simulation sets in which movements involving only a single individual or all ten individuals are common, but responses involving intermediate numbers of individuals are rare (Fig. 1B). That is, bimodality indicates the preponderance of two classes of outcome: failed initiations (when no movement occurs) and successful movements by the whole group.
Second, we compared simulation results to published field data of collective movements by Capuchin monkeys10,42. Capuchin monkeys are arboreal primates that form small groups (4–40 individuals) of multiple males/females of different ages and must collectively move between foraging sites frequently (see Methods for more details). The dataset used by Petit et al.10 comprised 255 collective movements of a group of Capuchin monkeys, which has been the focus of a series of theoretical and observational studies10,42,43, including the original model in Eqs. 1 and 2. In order to compare simulation results to field data, we quantify the differences between the distribution of responders (e.g., Fig. 1B) obtained from simulations and from field data by means of the Manhattan distance.
Simulation results
Assessment of bimodal response patterns
We observed pronounced variation response patterns between Bim and Uni conditions and democratic and despotic scenarios. Overall, the despotic scenario shows bimodal patterns of the number of responders more often than the democratic scenario. Under Uni conditions, bimodal response patterns were observed for both despotic and democratic scenarios only when considering low or no personality variation (α′ values ranging from ~10 to 1000), coupled with non-linear (or occasionally linear) feedback (Fig. 3A, B).
Under Bim conditions (Fig. 4A), response patterns in despotic groups were highly dependent on the number of leaders (individuals drawn from the low α′ distribution). In groups with few leaders, only weak bimodality was observed, and this was mostly at low levels of personality variation and with feedback present. The highest degrees of bimodality were observed for despotic–Bim scenarios with a balanced proportion of leaders and followers. Of these scenarios, those with the greatest differences in the two distributions of α′ and incorporating social feedback produced the most bimodal response patterns. Interestingly, however, scenarios with personality but without feedback mechanisms (Cα′) also resulted in bimodal response patterns over a large range of parameter values (Fig. 4A), indicating that such patterns can arise from within-group differences in personality alone. With high numbers of leaders, weak to moderate bimodal response patterns could be produced with or without social feedback. Higher bimodality was observed with increasing distance between the distributions of α′.
For the democratic scenario, bimodality was rare, and observed only in cases with low numbers of leaders, limited variation in α′, and feedback in place. In such case, Uni condition (Fig. 3B) leads to bimodality for a wider range of parameters compared to Bim condition (Fig. 4B). No matter how personality is distributed in a democratic (bottom-up) system, increasing the personality variation leads to low rate of bimodality.
Comparison to field data
We compared the distribution of the number of individuals involved in each movement generated by our simulation to that observed for a group of Capuchin monkeys in the field. For Uni conditions, there was only weak matching between simulations and field data, with the closest comparisons being found for simulations with non-linear feedback and low-mid variability in α′. Notably, however, the best-fitting parameter combinations for simulations with personality but no feedback (Cα′) achieved a similar degree of fit to the field data as the results of feedback-based simulations in both despotic and democratic scenarios (Fig. 5).
Simulations under Bim conditions generally produced a better fit to field data than Uni conditions, though this varied depending on the number of leaders, degree of variability in α′ and between despotic and democratic scenarios. The broadest fit of simulations to natural patterns was produced under a despotic scenario with non-linear feedback (Fig. 6A). However, the despotic scenario with no feedback (Cα′) also closely modelled field data for intermediate numbers of leaders. The democratic scenario generally produced a poorer fit to field data (Fig. 6B), though interestingly showed the greatest consistency with the despotic scenario for personality-only (Cα′) conditions.
As a further comparison between simulations and field data, we plotted frequency distributions of simulation outcomes against the field data of Petit et al.10 for key simulation results (Fig. 7). The scenario Bim–despotic with a balanced proportion of leaders:followers (5:5) reproduced the distribution of responders involved in natural movement dynamics as well as a model considering identical individuals with both feedback mechanisms (Petit model) (Fig. 7A, B). Interestingly, a despotic scenario with no feedback (Cα′) closely modelled field data for particular numbers of leaders (see Figs. 5A and 7C). In fact, the scenario Cα′ did a better job replicating the field data than the control models with individuals having the same rate of joining (α′ = 160) and with non-linear, linear or constant PC (Fig. 7D–F). This indicates that variability in α′ alone can produce patterns equivalent to those typically assumed to be driven by feedback mechanisms.
Analytical model
As a final step, we sought to clarify the relative importance of feedback and inter-individual variability through development of an analytical model68. We used this to test the hypothesis that systems of identical individuals could never produce a bimodal (J or U shape) response distribution without feedback, or if limited to linear feedback. This model initially assumes that there is no feedback between initiators and followers, and thus that the cancellation rate is constant. The master equation, which represents the counterpart of stochastic multi-agent simulations above, is as follows (see Supplementary Material for more details):
The state of the system is described in terms of a probability function Θ(i, k, t). At time t, i is the number of individuals who have started to follow the initiator (0 ≤ i ≤ N, where N is the total number of potential followers) and k = 1 if the initiator is still moving or k = 0 if the initiator cancelled its movement. Equation 7 describes the change over time of the probability that the system (Θ(i, k, t)) occupies each one of a discrete set of states. We define state as the number of responders to a given initiation, with the initiator still on the move or having cancelled the movement. Ψ(i, 1; i + 1, 1) is the probability of a transition from i to i + 1 responders (a new individual joins the movement), while Ψ(i, 1; i, 0) is the probability of transition from a state with i responders to the state in which the initiator cancels the movement. These transitions depend on both the probability of joining (PJ), defined by the identity of the initiator (despotic system), and the initiator’s probability of cancelling the movement (PC).
Constant probability of cancelling (\({P_{C}}\)) and following (\({P_{J}}\))
In the case where the PC and PJ of the initiator are constant:
and with identical individuals, the probability of transition from i to i + 1 responders is:
with N − i being the number of potential followers still at rest. Under these conditions, and with a time limit as specified for our simulations, the final distribution of group responses with Eq. 7 could never be bimodal. When extending the model to infinite-time, thus Θ(i, 1, ∞) = 0, the total distribution of the number of followers ΘT(i, ∞) = Θ(i, 0, ∞):
with a = PJ / PC. In such case, the distribution of responders to a given initiation was never bimodal, although this distribution was either skewed towards higher (if PJ > PC) or lower (if PJ < PC) numbers of responders depending on the PJ/PC ratio. If PJ = PC, the distribution was flat (Eq. 9a). With infinite-time, 1/PC and 1/PJ are respectively the mean duration of the movement and the mean duration before a follower joins the movement. Thus, the mean fraction of followers corresponds to:
In the case of personality, and with infinite-time, each initiator is characterised by its own value ak (PC and/or the PJ differ among individuals). The global distribution of the number of followers is:
where fk is the fraction of initiations characterised by a value ak.
Under a despotic system (Eq. 10), numerical analyses show that the minimum condition required to produce a bimodal outcome is to have a proportion of the group with a ratio PJ/PC < 1 and another proportion with a ratio PJ/PC > 1. This condition can produce a U-shape distribution of the number of responders to each initiation (maximum values at 0 and N, with the minimum value in between; see methods and Supplementary Figs. S7 and S8). Under a democratic scenario, there was no numerical solution giving a bimodal outcome (see Supplementary Material).
Probability of cancelling (\({P_{C}}\)) decreases with the number of followers (linear feedback)
Subsequently, we tested the behaviour of the analytical model when introducing a linear feedback in the initiator’s PC with unlimited time:
where PC0 is the intrinsic probability of cancelling, and i is the number of followers. In this case, and without differences between individuals, the probability of transition between state (i, 1) and (i, 0), which means cancelling a movement, is:
The probability of a transition from state (i, 1) to state (i + 1, 1) (i.e., an individual joins the movement and the initiator is still on the go) is:
For long t, thus Θ(ð, 1) = 0 and ΘT(ð, 0) = Θ(ð, 0, ∞), the probability distribution of responders to a given initiation is given by:
with
This model predicts that the mean fraction of followers increases with the total number of potential followers and the ratio PJ/PC (Fig. 8A), and as such is approximated by:
with γ ≈ 0.6, γ′ ≈ 1.1.
In addition, we analysed the probability distribution of followers (ΘT(i)) according to a (ratio PJ/PC). When the ratio PJ/PC was very large, the distribution of ΘT(i) was monomodal, with individuals always following the initiation (ΘTN = 1). When the ratio PJ/PC was very small, the distribution of ΘT(i) was also monomodal: ΘT0 = 1. These results are comparable with the results obtained without feedback in PC (Eq. 9a). Interestingly, between these two extreme situations, the distribution of ΘT(i) can be bimodal, depending on the ratio PJ/PC. If PJ is larger than PC, then ΘTN > ΘT0 and vice versa (Fig. 8B). These results indicate that at least some sort of feedback between initiator and responders is needed to generate a bimodal distribution when groups are composed of identical individuals.
Finally, we observe that the value of the ratio PJ/PC at which we observe the maximum bimodality decreases with the size of the group (see Supplementary Fig. S9). This means that when increasing the number of potential followers, the maximum bimodality is achieved with higher differences between PJ and PC values. The critical PJ/PC value decreases with about N–0.6 (see Supplementary Fig. S10).
Discriminating between personality and social effects
The analytical solutions above provide the basis for the following methods to discriminate between response patterns driven by feedback (social interactions) or personality variation. To facilitate such discrimination, we propose the following three methods. By limiting ourselves to the final distribution of responses as above, and if we can identify individuals, the computation of the number of responders according to the identity of the initiator using a non-parametric test would show whether some individuals are more often followed than others. If the identity of individuals is unknown, but it is possible to carry out observations on groups of different sizes, the mean fraction of responders (<f>) is independent (or quasi-independent) from the total population size when individuals show personality variation and there is no feedback:
where qi is the fraction of initiations by individuals with the value ai, with a = PJ/PC.
If the information required for the above is not available, we can still discern between mechanisms through the study of dynamics. Let us consider two extreme cases; on the one hand two types of initiators with different PC, and on the other hand a species without personality but with negative feedback influencing their cancellation probability PC. The collective movement dynamics of the group can be divided among several states of i moving individuals and ti (seconds) duration. The states with i moving individuals can only end with the addition of a new responder or by the cancellation of the movement by the initiator. If the distribution of durations for i responders corresponds to an exponential law, the average duration <T> of the state of i moving individuals for the species with feedback is:
and the standard deviation is also equal to <T>. Therefore, the coefficient of variation (CV) is = 1. In the case of a species with personality, the mean time <T> corresponds to:
lm being the number of initiators (≤N), each with its own PC. In such case, the variance is:
If Tl differs (i.e., PC or PJ differ among individuals), the coefficient of variation will be larger than 1. In addition, a survival curve analysis of the state, which depends on the number of individuals already departed, can be used as a proxy to estimate whether we observe a feedback or personality-related process. If the survival function is different from an exponential law, the process is likely to be under the influence of personality. If, on the other hand, the distribution fits an exponential function, it is suggestive of a social feedback-based mechanism. Thus, the coefficient of variance, combined with the analysis of survival curves, could give us information on whether the process underlying a collective action is mainly driven by social feedback or personality differences. The supposition that CV = 1 in groups of identical (or almost identical) individuals is theoretical and is based on the assumption of a high number of observations without memory or learning effects. Thus, the analysis of natural observations might show some variation from the expectation of CV = 1. It may be possible to statistically test the deviance of experimental observations from a theoretical CV = 1 as follows. First, we compute the average duration <T> and CV. We then perform n simulations, with n being the number of observations, and compute the CV of those n simulations for states of 0, 1, 2 … N – 1 followers (N being the group size). After repeating this process many times (e.g., 1000 or 10,000) we can generate a theoretical distribution of CV under the assumption that individuals are identical. If the experimental CV is not statistically different from this distribution, we can rule out personality effects.
Discussion
Many theoretical studies have shown that signal and cue-based feedback mechanisms in a wide variety of group-living organisms underpin a range of collective actions, from the movement decisions of monkeys69 to the foraging and emigration behaviour of large colonies of social insects4,59,70. However, few of these studies have incorporated inter-individual differences in behavioural propensity (i.e., animal personality), despite the growing evidence that this phenomenon is important in determining the outcome of collective actions18,32,71,72. Our results clearly show that while linear or non-linear feedback mechanisms can drive movement decisions in small groups of animals, the same collective patterns can arise without feedback mechanisms under certain scenarios if individuals vary in a single personality parameter. While previous studies have explored the effects of adding personality components to feedback-based models27,43, our results are the first to our knowledge to demonstrate that personality can entirely replace feedback mechanisms in collective actions. While it is likely that in nature both of these mechanisms interact to generate the patterns of collective behaviour, this highlights the potential importance of personality as an alternative explanatory mechanism, where feedback-based responses are typically assumed to hold the predominant role. This is particularly pertinent considering the wealth of recent evidence for the existence of personality in diverse taxa, from those living in small groups73,74,75,76 to large self-organised societies21,26,71.
While our results show that both despotic and democratic scenarios could generate a bimodal distribution of moving individuals, the despotic scenario was effective at doing so over a broader parameter space. Furthermore, only the despotic scenario could model natural groups effectively in the absence of feedback (i.e., PC constant and Cα′). The democratic scenario produced a bimodal response distribution only when considering non-linear feedback and low inter-individual variability (democratic–Bim/Uni with non-linear PC). These results, together with the mathematical model, suggest that personality alone cannot give rise to effective collective actions when driven from the bottom-up. On the other hand, the despotic scenario was effective over various combinations of parameter values without feedback in place. This may indicate that democratic systems are likely to rely more on feedback in collective decision making, whereas despotic systems can function without. It is notable that increasing differences between individuals reduces the relative importance of feedback in both scenarios (Uni/Bim) and that these feedback become necessary to replicate natural patterns when considering identical individuals. Indeed, a bimodal response pattern never arose in the absence of feedback with identical individuals in simulations, and this was confirmed by our analytical model (see also10). Based on these theoretical findings, we speculate that large societies such as social insects, often considered exemplary democratic systems, should rely strongly on feedback and exhibit limited influence of personality. On the other hand, we might expect to find a larger influence of personality in small societies, even without well-established social hierarchies, where shared decisions may be driven by top-down, despotic, mechanisms. Quantifying individual variability in societies of different sizes and with clear democratic or despotic systems during collective decision making would help to test this prediction.
Our simulations indicate that bimodal outcomes were produced over a broader range of parameters by groups with a bimodal distribution of personalities than under Uni conditions. The same was true of the match between simulation outcomes and the natural patterns of collective movement in Capuchin monkeys. It is worth noting that there is a fundamental difference between Bim and Uni conditions, in that sets of individuals drawn from the Bim distribution will always have bimodally distributed personalities, while those drawn from the Uni distribution will form different distributions of α′ each time. We might thus expect consistently bimodal patterns in the collective movements of Bim groups, while Uni groups will give rise to different response distributions in each simulation depending on the distribution of α′ values. The success of our Uni scenario in producing bimodal patterns under some conditions may thus be in part an artefact of replication, in that different parts of the distribution of responses will be emphasised in each replicate, which in combination can generate the bimodal response pattern. This may thus be a poor approximation of success on a case-by-case basis, as might be expected in nature. Thus, the limited success of unimodally distributed personalities we find may actually overstate the effectiveness of this distribution of personalities, adding further weight to our finding that bimodality enhances success.
The fact that bimodal distributions of personality most effectively approximate the behaviour of natural groups with or without feedback is perhaps not surprising. Several studies have shown that groups composed by heterogeneous personalities, or a mixture of phenotypes, perform better than homogeneous groups13,31,32,33. Furthermore, several theoretical models predict a stable bimodal distribution of leadership23,65,77 depending on density, context and benefits associated with each strategy65,78. In addition, our results indicate that the best approximations of natural patterns were found in groups in which there was not only a bimodal distribution of personalities, but also an approximate numerical balance in personality types (a ratio of 5:5 leaders:followers, or 7:3 in Cα′). This is in agreement with a recent model suggesting that balanced distribution of personalities can increase the rate of successful movements in small groups43,79. The fact that groups with balanced, bimodal distributions of personality can better replicate natural dynamics raises an important question: does this mean that groups adaptively tend towards such compositions to optimise collective actions? If so, we might expect regulation mechanisms to exist which facilitate compositions of distinct leaders and followers or different extreme phenotypes. Optimal distributions could be a product of selection against individuals within groups with ineffective compositions, which either fail or disband as a result19,24,80. Alternatively, adjustment of personalities to the optimal composition could arise through social conformity and behavioural plasticity81 or through admittance or exclusion of new individuals by other group members based on their suitability for forming an optimal distribution of personalities24.
Our findings indicate that personality is a potentially important component of collective behaviour, and can act synergistically with, or possibly replace, mechanisms of positive/negative feedback. In our model, these effects were an emergent property of variation in a single personality characteristic. This characteristic could represent various behavioural traits, such as boldness27, motivational state29,30, social status53 or even vocalisation intensity82,83 and as such, our findings may be applicable to a broad range of group-living organisms. Future studies could explore the influence of additional personality parameters, as this may lead to synergistic or stabilising effects resulting in more complex group-level responses. It would also be valuable to assess the relative importance of personality and feedback mechanisms in generating collective behaviours in different taxa with differing social systems. To this end, we demonstrate several methods that could help elucidate the relative importance of personality and social effects during collective actions, even when the identity of individuals is unknown. We suggest that discrimination between these effects requires data with accurate timing of events, and can be facilitated by using marked individuals where possible. This framework will help improve our understanding on the relative importance of personality variation, positive/negative feedback, social bonds and hierarchies in generating collective behaviours in different kinds of group-living organisms and help answer the question of which, if any, of these components is a universal driver of collective behaviour.
Methods
Subject of study
Capuchin monkeys are arboreal primates who form persistent groups of 4–40 individuals84 composed of multiple males and females of different ages, and with high degree of relatedness within groups. Females usually remain in the same group, while males tend to migrate to new social groups85. These monkeys forage on several types of plants and trees, looking for fruits and small animals or insects85, and must collectively move between foraging sites frequently. The experimental data used in this study come from a semi-free-range group of monkeys established in 1989 at the Primatology Centre of the Louis Pasteur University, Strasbourg, France. This group was subject of several experimental and theoretical studies10,42,43. The group was composed by five males aged of 2, 6, 6, 7 and >20 years old and five females; two of 5, one of 7 and two of >20 years old. Individuals of more than 20 years old were wild-born, while others were born in captivity.
Simulations
Simulations were performed in Python 2.786. We considered 16 scenarios in total (Fig. 2). For Bim/Uni we considered different ranges of variability: 117 different ranges of α′ for Bim (160: 3960) and 132 for Uni (10: 4460). For Bim (despotic/democratic) we considered different proportions of follower:leaders (high:low α′) within groups9 for the full range of α′. We performed 5000 simulations for each unique combination of parameters using seconds as unit of time. In total we performed 11,850,000 simulations (2370 unique parameter combinations).
Bimodality index
In order to classify a distribution of a collective movement of N individuals as bimodal, the frequency values for the extremes of the distribution (1 and N) should be higher than those for intermediate values, as these extremes respectively correspond to situations where the initiator cancelled after attracting no followers, or was successful in initiating a group movement (all individuals moved). There are several indexes to statistically determine if response distributions could be considered bimodal87. To stress the differences between the frequency of the extreme values (1 and 10 followers), we calculated a bimodality index (BI) as follows. First, we checked that the extremes (in this case 1 and 10 responders) correspond to the two maximum values. Second, we took the lower number of observations of the two extreme values of the distribution (1 and 10) and calculated the ratio between this and the mean number of observations for all other number of responders (i.e., 2 to N – 1 agents). Therefore, an index of 2 means that the lowest of the two extreme values of the distribution (either 1 or 10) was twice the mean of the frequencies of all other numbers of responders [2 – 9 agents] (see Fig. 1B). For the analytical model section, the computation of BI was simplified and we used:
where As is the amplitude of the smaller peak of the two extreme values of the distribution (i.e., 0 and 10), and Am is the amplitude of the minimum value of the distribution88.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available within the paper and Supplementary Information. Additional data are available on request from the corresponding author.
Code availability
The simulation code can be found in the Supplementary Information.
References
Boinski, S. & Garber, P. A. On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups (University of Chicago Press, 2000).
Amé, J.-M., Halloy, J., Rivault, C., Detrain, C. & Deneubourg, J. L. Collegial decision making based on social amplification leads to optimal group formation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 103, 5835–5840 (2006).
Bonabeau, E., Theraulaz, G., Deneubourg, J.-L., Aron, S. & Camazine, S. Self-organization in social insects. TREE 12, 188–193 (1997).
Camazine, S. et al. Self-Organization in Biological Systems (Princeton University Press, 2001).
Conradt, L. & Roper, T. J. Democracy in animals: the evolution of shared group decisions. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 274, 2317–2326 (2007).
Deneubourg, J.-L. & Goss, S. Collective patterns and decision-making. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 1, 295–311 (1989).
Jeanson, R., Dussutour, A. & Fourcassié, V. Key factors for the emergence of collective decision in invertebrates. Front. Neurosci. 6, 1–15 (2012).
Hemelrijk, C. K. (ed.) Self-Organisation and Evolution of Social Systems (Cambrige University Press, 2005).
King, A. J. & Sueur, C. Where next? Group coordination and collective decision making by primates. Int. J. Primatol. 32, 1245–1267 (2011).
Petit, O., Gautrais, J., Leca, J.-B., Theraulaz, G. & Deneubourg, J.-L. Collective decision-making in White-faced capuchin monkeys. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 3495–3503 (2009).
Darwin, C. 1968 The Origin of Species (New York, NY Mod. Libr., 1859).
Hölldobler, B. & Wilson, E. O. The Superorganism (W. W. Norton, 2008).
Mattila, H. R. & Seeley, T. D. Genetic diversity in honey bee colonies enhances productivity and fitness. Science 317, 362–364 (2007).
Dall, S. R. X. & Griffith, S. C. An empiricist guide to animal personality variation in ecology and evolution. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2, 3 (2014).
Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., Mcdougall, P. T. & Dingemanse, N. J. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol. Rev. 82, 291–318 (2007).
Sih, A., Bell, A. & Johnson, J. C. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 372–378 (2004).
Carere, C. & Maestripieri, D. (eds) Animal Personalities: Behavior, Physiology, and Evolution (University of Chicago Press, 2013).
Webster, M. M. & Ward, A. J. W. Personality and social context. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 86, 759–773 (2011).
Wright, C. M. et al. Collective personalities: present knowledge and new frontiers. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 73, 1–23 (2019).
Bengston, S. E. & Jandt, J. M. The development of collective personality: the ontogenetic drivers of behavioral variation across groups. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2, 1–13 (2014).
Jeanson, R. & Weidenmüller, A. Interindividual variability in social insects—proximate causes and ultimate consequences. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 89, 671–687 (2014).
Beekman, M. & Jordan, L. A. Does the field of animal personality provide any new insights for behavioral ecology? Behav. Ecol. 28, 617–623 (2017).
Couzin, I. D., Krause, J., Franks, N. R. & Levin, S. A. Effective leadership and decision- making in animal groups on the move. Nature 433, 513–516 (2005).
Farine, D. R., Montiglio, P.-O. & Spiegel, O. From individuals to groups and back: the evolutionary implications of group phenotypic composition. Trends Ecol. Evol. 30, 609–621 (2015).
Ioannou, C. C. & Dall, S. R. X. Individuals that are consistent in risk-taking benefit during collective foraging. Sci. Rep. 6, 33991 (2016).
Jandt, J. M. et al. Behavioural syndromes and social insects: personality at multiple levels. Biol. Rev. 89, 48–67 (2014).
Jolles, J. W., Boogert, N. J., Sridhar, V. H., Couzin, I. D. & Manica, A. Consistent individual differences drive collective behavior and group functioning of schooling fish. Curr. Biol. 27, 2862–2868.e7 (2017).
King, A. J., Johnson, D. D. P. & Van, M. Vugt, The origins and evolution of leadership. Curr. Biol. 19, R911–R916 (2009).
Gérard, C., Valenchon, M. Poulin, N. & Petit, O. How does the expressiveness of leaders affect followership in domestic horses (Equus ferus caballus)? Anim. Cogn. 23, 559–569 (2020).
Ceccarelli, E., Negrín, A. R., Coyohua‐Fuentes, A., Canales‐Espinosa, D. & Dias, P. A. D. Sex differences in leadership during group movement in mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Am. J. Primatol. 82, e23099 (2020).
Dyer, J. R. G., Croft, D. P., Morrell, L. J. & Krause, J. Shoal composition determines foraging success in the guppy. Behav. Ecol. 20, 165–171 (2009).
Sih, A. & Watters, J. V. The mix matters: behavioural types and group dynamics in water striders. Behaviour 142, 1417–1431 (2005).
Modlmeier, A. P. & Foitzik, S. Productivity increases with variation in aggression among group members in Temnothorax ants. Behav. Ecol. 22, 1026–1032 (2011).
Harcourt, J. L., Sweetman, G., Johnstone, R. A. & Manica, A. Personality counts: the effect of boldness on shoal choice in three-spined sticklebacks. Anim. Behav. 77, 1501–1505 (2009).
Sumpter, D. Collective Animal Behavior (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 2010).
Couzin, I. D. & Krause, J. Self-organization and collective behavior in vertebrates. Adv. Study Behav. 32, 1–75 (2003).
King, A. J. & Cowlishaw, G. Leaders, followers and group decision-making. Commun. Integr. Biol. 2, 147–150 (2009).
Strandburg-Peshkin, A., Farine, D. R., Couzin, I. D. & Crofoot, M. C. Shared decision-making drives collective movement in wild baboons. Science 348, 1358–1361 (2015).
Black, J. M. Preflight signalling in swans: a mechanism for group cohesion and flock formation. Ethology 79, 143–157 (1988).
Seeley, T. D. & Visscher, P. K. Group decision making in nest-site selection by honey bees. Apidologie 35, 101–116 (2004).
Danchin, É., Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T. J. & Wagner, R. H. Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305, 487–491 (2004).
Leca, J. B., Gunst, N., Thierry, B. & Petit, O. Distributed leadership in semifree-ranging white-faced capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 66, 1045–1052 (2003).
Eskridge, B. & Schlupp, I. Effects of personality distribution on collective behavior. in Artificial Life 14: Proceedings of the Fourteenth International Conference on the Synthesis and Simulation of Living Systems (eds Hiroki, S., John, R., Sebastian R., René D. & Hod, L.) pp. 908–915 (The MIT Press, 2014).
Ward, A. J. W., Sumpter, D. J. T., Couzin, I. D., Hart, P. J. B. & Krause, J. Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 105, 6948–6953 (2008).
Reebs, S. G. Can a minority of informed leaders determine the foraging movements of a fish shoal? Anim. Behav. 59, 403–409 (2000).
Ward, A. J. W. et al. Initiators, leaders, and recruitment mechanisms in the collective movements of damselfish. Am. Nat. 181, 748–760 (2013).
Wagner, R. H. & Danchin, E. Conspecific copying: a general mechanism of social aggregation. Anim. Behav. 2, 405–408 (2003).
Kummer, H. Social organization of hamadryas baboons. A field study. (University of Chicago, Chicago, 1968).
Dumont, B., Boissy, A., Achard, C., Sibbald, A. M. & Erhard, H. W. Consistency of animal order in spontaneous group movements allows the measurement of leadership in a group of grazing heifers. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 95, 55–66 (2005).
Ramseyer, A., Thierry, B., Boissy, A. & Dumont, B. Decision‐making processes in group departures of cattle. Ethology 115, 948–957 (2009).
Bourjade, M., Thierry, B., Maumy, M. & Petit, O. Decision‐making in Przewalski horses (Equus ferus przewalskii) is driven by the ecological contexts of collective movements. Ethology 115, 321–330 (2009).
Meunier, H., Leca, J. B., Deneubourg, J.-L. & Petit, O. Group movement decisions in capuchin monkeys. Behaviour 143, 1511–1527 (2006).
King, A. J., Sueur, C., Huchard, E. & Cowlishaw, G. A rule-of-thumb based on social affiliation explains collective movements in desert baboons. Anim. Behav. 82, 1337–1345 (2011).
Ramseyer, A., Boissy, A., Thierry, B. & Dumont, B. Individual and social determinants of spontaneous group movements in cattle and sheep. Animal 3, 1319–1326 (2009).
Briard, L., Dorn, C. & Petit, O. Personality and affinities play a key role in the organisation of collective movements in a group of domestic horses. Ethology 121, 888–902 (2015).
Sueur, C. & Petit, O. Organization of group members at departure is driven by social structure in Macaca. Int. J. Primatol. 29, 1085–1098 (2008).
Petit, O. & Bon, R. Decision-making processes: the case of collective movements. Behav. Process. 84, 635–647 (2010).
Conradt, L. & Roper, T. J. Consensus decision making in animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 449–456 (2005). Personal Ed.
Cronin, A. L. An agent-based model of nest-site selection in a mass-recruiting ant. J. Theor. Biol. 455, 54–63 (2018).
Dall, S. R. X., Houston, A. I. & McNamara, J. M. The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecol. Lett. 7, 734–739 (2004).
Pinter-Wollman, N. Personality in social insects: how does worker personality determine colony personality? Curr. Zool. 58, 580–588 (2012).
Jandt, J. M. & Dornhaus, A. Bumblebee response thresholds and body size: does worker diversity increase colony performance? Anim. Behav. 87, 97–106 (2014).
Weidenmüller, A. The control of nest climate in bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) colonies: interindividual variability and self reinforcement in fanning response. Behav. Ecol. 15, 120–128 (2004).
Carere, C., Audebrand, C., Rödel, H. G. & d’Ettorre, P. Individual behavioural type and group performance in Formica fusca ants. Behav. Process. 157, 402–407 (2018).
Hemelrijk, C. K. & Wantia, J. Individual variation by self-organisation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 125–136 (2005).
King, A. J., Douglas, C. M. S., Huchard, E., Isaac, N. J. B. & Cowlishaw, G. Dominance and affiliation mediate despotism in a social primate. Curr. Biol. 18, 1833–1838 (2008).
Amé, J.-M., Holloy, J., Rivault, C., Detrain, C. & Deneubourg, J. L. Collegial decision making based on social amplification leads to optimal group formation. PNAS 103, 5835–5840 (2006).
Nicolis, G. & Baras, F. Intrinsic randomness and spontaneous symmetry-breaking in explosive systems. J. Stat. Phys. 48, 1071–1090 (1987).
Krause, J. & Ruxton, G. Living in Groups (Oxford University Press, 2002).
Pratt, S. C., Mallon, E. B., Sumpter, D. J. T. & Franks, N. R. Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52, 117–127 (2002).
Cronin, A. L. Individual and group personalities characterise consensus decision-making in an ant. Ethology 121, 703–713 (2015).
Planas-Sitjà, I., Deneubourg, J.-L., Gibon, C. & Sempo, G. Group personality during collective decision-making: a multi-level approach. Proc. R. Soc. B 282 (2015).
Brown, C. & Irving, E. Individual personality traits influence group exploration in a feral guppy population. Behav. Ecol. 25, 95–101 (2013).
Dussutour, A., Nicolis, S. C., Despland, E. & Simpson, S. J. Individual differences influence collective behaviour in social caterpillars. Anim. Behav. 76, 5–16 (2008).
Michelena, P., Jeanson, R., Deneubourg, J.-L. & Sibbald, A. M. Personality and collective decision-making in foraging herbivores. Proc. Biol. Sci. 277, 1093–1099 (2010).
Kurvers, R. H. J. M. et al. The effect of personality on social foraging: shy barnacle geese scrounge more. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B-Biol. Sci. 277, 601–608 (2010).
Guttal, V. & Couzin, I. D. Social interactions, information use, and the evolution of collective migration. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 107, 16172–16177 (2010).
Smith, J. E. et al. Leadership in mammalian societies: emergence, distribution, power, and payoff. Trends Ecol. Evol. 31, 54–66 (2016).
Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Mann, R. P. & Sheldon, B. C. Individual-level personality influences social foraging and collective behaviour in wild birds. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281, 20141016 (2014).
Wolf, J. B., Brodie, E. D. III & Moore, A. J. Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process. II. Selection resulting from social interactions. Am. Nat. 153, 254–266 (1999).
King, A. J., Williams, L. J. & Mettke-Hofmann, C. The effects of social conformity on Gouldian finch personality. Anim. Behav. 99, 25–31 (2015).
Bousquet, C. A. H., Sumpter, D. J. T. & Manser, M. B. Moving calls: a vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 1482–1488 (2011).
Stueckle, S. & Zinner, D. To follow or not to follow: decision making and leadership during the morning departure in chacma baboons. Anim. Behav. 75, 1995–2004 (2008).
Fragaszy, D. M., Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. M. The Complete Capuchin: The Biology of the Genus Cebus (Cambridge University Press, 2004).
Panger, M. A. et al. Cross-site differences in foraging behavior of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 119, 52–66 (2002).
Python Software Foundation. Python Language Reference, version 2.7. Available at https://www.python.org.
Knapp, T. R. Bimodality revisited. J. Mod. Appl. Stat. Methods 6, 3 (2007).
Zhang, C., Mapes, B. E. & Soden, B. J. Bimodality in tropical water vapour. Q. J. R. Meteorol. Soc. A J. Atmos. Sci. Appl. Meteorol. Phys. Oceanogr. 129, 2847–2866 (2003).
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) and Tokyo Metropolitan University.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
I. P.-S. and J.-L. D. conceived the original idea. I. P.-S. wrote the first draft of the manuscript, carried out the simulations, analyses and graphical representation. A. L. C. helped develop the original idea, supervised the project and contributed to the final manuscript. J.-L. D. developed the analytical model and contributed to the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Planas-Sitjà, I., Deneubourg, JL. & Cronin, A.L. Variation in personality can substitute for social feedback in coordinated animal movements. Commun Biol 4, 469 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-021-01991-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-021-01991-9
- Springer Nature Limited
This article is cited by
-
Growth and physiological response of proactive and reactive juvenile “tambaqui” (Colossoma macropomum) in a recirculating aquaculture system
Tropical Animal Health and Production (2024)
-
Familiarity, age, weaning and health status impact social proximity networks in dairy calves
Scientific Reports (2023)