Skip to main content
Log in

Social Justice in Our Minds, Homes, and Society: The Nature, Causes, and Consequences of Implicit Bias

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Social injustice is disruptive both personally and collectively, and the ordinariness of implicit prejudice plays a role in this process. In this report, I discuss the correspondence between implicit and explicit biases, and factors that moderate their association. In addition, I differentiate between declarative and procedural definitions of implicit bias, which have implications for their nonconscious nature. To underscore their conceptual distinction, I also present evidence that points to different origins for implicit and explicit biases. Finally, actions for confronting implicit bias are prescribed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice, Addison-Wesley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, E., and Massey, D. S. (2001). Problem of the Century: Racial Stratification in the United States, Russell Sage Foundation, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashburn-Nardo, L., Knowles, M. L., and Monteith, M. J. (2003). Black Americans' implicit racial associations and their implications for intergroup judgment. Soc. Cogn. 21: 61-87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., and Monteith, M. J. (2001). Implicit associations as the seeds of intergroup bias: How easily do they take root? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81: 789-799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitudes can be measured. In Roediger, H. L., Nairne, J. S., Neath, I., and Surprenant, A. M. (eds.), The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G. Crowder, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 117-150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banse, R., Seise, J., and Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes toward homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Z. Exp. Psychol. 48: 1145-1160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In Uleman, J. S., and Bargh, J. A. (eds.), Unintended Thought, Guilford, New York, pp. 3-51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, I. V. (2001). Implicit stereotypes and prejudice. In Moskowitz, G. B. (ed.), Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 359-374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6: 242-261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boniecki, K. A., and Jacks, J. Z. (2002). The elusive relationship between measures of implicit and explicit prejudice. Representative Res. Soc. Psychol. 26: 1-14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cokerinos, L. (2003). The Assault on Diversity: An Organized Challenge to Racial and Gender Justice, Rowman and Littlefield, Lanham, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., and Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, reliability, and convergent validity. Psychol. Sci. 121: 163-170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N. (2004). Implicit ingroup favoritism, outgroup favoritism, and their behavioral manifestations. Soc. Justice Res. 17(2): 143-168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., and Asgari, S. (2003). Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereotyping and self-stereotyping. Manuscript submitted for review.

  • Dasgupta, N., and Asgari, S. (in press). Seeing is believing: Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereotyping. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol.

  • Dasgupta, N., and Rivera, L. (2003). The road sometimes taken: How negative affect, perceiver sex, and target sex influence the path from implicit sexual prejudice to behavior. Manuscript in preparation.

  • Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56: 5-18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., and Beach, K. R. (2001). Implicit and explicit attitudes: Examination of the relationship between measures of intergroup bias. In Brown, R., and Gaertner, S. L. (eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology, Vol. 4: Intergroup Relations, Blackwell, Oxford, UK, pp. 175-197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., and Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interactions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82: 62-68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., and Howard, A. (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 33: 510-540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunton, B. C., and Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23: 316-326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., and Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychol. Rev. 109: 573-598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egloff, B., and Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of the Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83: 1441-1455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In Zanna, M. P. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 23, Academic Press, New York, pp. 75-109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., and Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes. A bona fide pipeline? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69: 1013-1027.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, R. H., and Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 54: 297-327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Row, Peterson and Company, Evanston, IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Florack, A., Scarabis, M., and Bless, H. (2001). When do associations matter? The use of associations toward ethnic groups in person judgments. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37: 518-524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, S. L., and Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In Dovidio, J. F., and Gaertner, S. L. (eds.), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism, Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 61-90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. Am. Psychol. 35: 603-618.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102: 4-27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., and Mellott, D. S. (2002a). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychol. Rev. 109: 3-25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74: 1464-1480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., and Nosek, B. A. (2001). The health of the Implicit Association Test at age 3. Z. Exp. Psychol. 48: 85-93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: An improved scoring algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85: 197-216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Pickrell, J. E., and Farnham, S. D. (2002b). Implicit partisanship: Taking sides for no reason. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83: 367-379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., and Banaji, M R. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system-justification and the production of false-consciousness. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 33: 1-27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., and Carvallo, M. R. (2002). Non-conscious forms of system justification: Implicit and behavioral preferences for higher status groups. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38: 586-602.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., Park, B., Ryan, C. S., Brauer, M., and Kraus, S. (1995). Stereotypes and ethnocentricism: Interethnic perceptions of African American and White American college samples. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69: 460-481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., and Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81: 774-788.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingston, R. W. (2002). The role of perceived negativity in the moderation of African Americans' implicit and explicit racial attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38: 405-413.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. In Dovidio, J. F., and Gaertner, S. L. (eds.), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism, Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 91-126.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, A. R., and Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, explicit attitudes, and discriminatory behavior. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37: 435-442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteith, M. J., Voils, C. I., and Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2001). Taking a look underground: Detecting, interpreting, and reacting to implicit biases. Soc. Cogn. 19: 395-417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moody, J. (2001). Race, school integration, and friendship segregation in America. Am. J. Soc. 107: 679-716.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2002). (At least) two factors moderate the relationship between implicit and explicit attitudes. In Ohme, R. K., and Jarymowicz, M. (eds.), Natura Automatyzmow, WIP PAN and SWPS, Warszawa, pp. 49-56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., and Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dyn. Theory Res. Pract. 6: 101-115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, M. A., and Fazio, R. H. (2002). Implicit acquisition and manifestation of classically conditioned attitudes. Soc. Cogn. 20: 89-104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., and Banaji, M. R. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12: 729-738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plant, E. A., and Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75: 811-832.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A. (2004). Sources of implicit attitudes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 13(2): 80-83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Ashmore, R. D., and Gary, M. L. (2001). “Unlearning” automatic biases: The malleability of implicit stereotypes and prejudice. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81: 856-868.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., and Borgida, E. (1995). The afterglow of construct accessibility: The behavioral consequences of priming men to view women as sexual objects. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 31: 493-517.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Feinberg, J. M., and Fairchild, K. (2002). Minority members' implicit attitudes: Ingroup bias as a function of group status. Soc. Cogn. 20: 294-320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., and Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women. J. Soc. Issues 57: 743-762.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., and Goodwin, S. A. (2003). [Priming male violence increases implicit pro-female bias]. Unpublished raw data.

  • Rudman, L. A., and Goodwin, S. A. (in press). Gender differences in automatic in-group bias: Why do women like women more than men like men? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.

  • Rudman, L. A., and Heppen, J. (2003). Implicit romantic fantasies and women's interest in personal power: A glass slipper effect? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29: 1357-1370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., and Kilianski, S. E. (2000). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward female authority. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26: 1315-1328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., and Lee, M. R. (2002). Implicit and explicit consequences of exposure to violent and misogynous rap music. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 5: 133-150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schuman, H., Steeh, C., Bobo, L., and Krysan, M. (1997). Racial Attitudes in America: Trends and Interpretations, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Towles-Schwen, T., and Fazio, R. H. (2003). Choosing social situations: The relation between automatically activated racial attitudes and anticipated comfort interacting with African Americans. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29: 170-182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., and Brekke, N. (1994). Mental contamination and mental correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychol. Bull. 116: 117-142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., and Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychol. Rev. 107: 101-126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., and Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with questionnaire measures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72: 262-274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., and Park, B. (2001). Evaluative versus conceptual judgments in automatic stereotyping and prejudice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37: 244-252.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laurie A. Rudman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rudman, L.A. Social Justice in Our Minds, Homes, and Society: The Nature, Causes, and Consequences of Implicit Bias. Social Justice Research 17, 129–142 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SORE.0000027406.32604.f6

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SORE.0000027406.32604.f6

Navigation