Skip to main content
Log in

Implicit Cognitive Distortions and Sexual Offending

  • Published:
Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment

    We’re sorry, something doesn't seem to be working properly.

    Please try refreshing the page. If that doesn't work, please contact support so we can address the problem.

Abstract

This work develops and tests the semantic-motivation hypothesis of sexual offenders' implicit cognitions. This hypothesis posits that sexual offenders' cognitive distortions emerge at the interface between implicit motivation and cognition. The semantic-motivation hypothesis is used to guide the development of 3 implicit association tests (IATs). These IATs were used to test for the existence of 3 expected child sexual offender implicit cognitive distortions in child sexual offenders (201C;children as sexual beings,201D; 201C;uncontrollability of sexuality,201D; and 201C;sexual entitlement-bias201D;). Results showed that child sexual offenders had larger IAT effects than did mainstream offenders and male and female nonoffenders for the 201C;children as sexual beings201D; and the 201C;uncontrollability of sexuality201D; implicit theories. Child sexual offenders also had a larger IAT effect than male and female nonoffenders for the 201C;sexual entitlement-bias201D; implicit theory. Implications for the semantic-motivation hypothesis are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Monteith, M. J. (2001). Implicit associations as the seeds of intergroup bias: How easily do they take root? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 ,789–799

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.) (pp. 680–740). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brett, M. (2002). Lexicate Online Dictionary List Retrieved from http://www.lexicate.com

  • Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology, 31 ,23–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., McGhee, D. E., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Automatic preference for White Americans: Eliminating the familiarity explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36 ,316–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2001). A structural and process analysis of the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37 ,443–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckhardt, C. H., Barbour, K. A., & Davison, G. C. (1998). Articulated thoughts of maritally violent and nonviolent men during anger arousal. Journal of Counselling and Clinical Psychology, 66 ,259–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, G. (1998). Multivariate analysis of variance. Melbourne: Swinburne University of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102 ,4–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109 ,3–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 ,1022–1037.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 ,1464–1480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., O'Brien, L. T., Greenwald, A. G., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). Using the implicit association test to measure age differences in implicit social cognition. Psychology and Aging, 17 ,482–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langton, C. M., & Marshall, W. L. (2000). The role of cognitive distortions in relapse prevention programs. In D. R. Laws, S. M. Hudson, & T. Ward (Eds.), Remaking relapse prevention with sexual offenders (pp. 167–186). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laws, D. R., Hudson, S. M., & Ward, T. (2000). Remaking relapse prevention with sex offenders. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, W. L., Anderson, D., & Fernandez, Y. (1999). Cognitive behavioural treatment of sexual offenders. Brisbane, Australia: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mihailides, S. P., Devilly, G. J., & Ward, T. (2004). Meaning and motivational systems in sexual offenders' cognitive distortions.Manuscript in preparation.

  • Millisecond Software. (2000). Inquisit [Computer software]. Seattle, WA: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., & Grafman, J. (2001). Vetromedial prefrontal cortex lesions in humans eliminate implicit gender stereotyping. Journal of Neuroscience, 21 ,1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, S. L., & Giacalone, R. A. (1990). Social psychological approaches to the perception of ethical dilemmas. Human Relations, 43 ,649–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., Mellott, D. S., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility and generality of the implicit association test. Social Cognition, 17 ,437–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strong, D. R., Greene, R. L., & Kordinak, S. T. (2002). Taxometric analysis of impression management and self-deception in college student and personnel evaluation settings. Journal of Personality Assessment, 78 ,161–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teachman, B. A., Gregg, A. P., & Woody, S. R. (2001). Implicit associations for fear-relevant stimuli among individuals with snake and spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110 ,226–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T. (2000). Sexual offenders' cognitive distortions as implicit theories. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5 ,491–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., Johnston, L., & Marshall, W. L. (1997). Cognitive distortions in sex offenders: An integrative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 17 ,479–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, T., & Keenan, T. (1999). Child molesters' implicit theories. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14 ,821–838.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen Mihailides.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mihailides, S., Devilly, G.J. & Ward, T. Implicit Cognitive Distortions and Sexual Offending. Sex Abuse 16, 333–350 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SEBU.0000043327.10168.5b

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:SEBU.0000043327.10168.5b

Navigation