Skip to main content
Log in

Anthropomorphism and Animism in Early Years Science: Why Teachers Use Them, how They Conceptualise Them and What Are Their Views on Their Use

  • Published:
Research in Science Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is considerable evidence that use of anthropomorphism and animism in science teaching is a common practice in all grades of education. However, not much is known about teachers' own views on the real reasons why they have been using animistic and anthropomorphic formulations or on the issue of whether animism and anthropomorphism should or should not be used in science. The present work, which was carried out in Greece, investigates early years teachers' views on the use of animism and anthropomorphism and on the reasons behind their use of these formulations. The study was designed as a small-scale exploration study. Research data were obtained from recorded group interviews and from written tasks. Results indicate that early years teachers seem to adopt the view that animism and anthropomorphism in early years science can cause cognitive problems in young children, and also that these teachers believe that in special cases use of animism and anthropomorphism can cause emotional problems as well. Results also reveal that, despite their reservations, teachers use animism and anthropomorphism both consciously and unconsciously and that they attribute their conscious use of these formulations to their low levels of content and pedagogical content knowledge in science.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bell, C. R. (1954). Additional data on animistic thinking. Scientific Monthly, 79, 67–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L., & Manion, L. (1997). Research methods of education. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crannell, C. N. (1954). Responses of college students to a questionnaire on animistic thinking. Scientific Monthly, 78, 54–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowell, D. H., & Dole, A. A. (1957). Animism and college students. Journal of Educational Research, 50, 391–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, W. (1953). Animistic thinking among college and university students. Scientific Monthly, 76, 247–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, W. (1957). Animistic thinking among college and high school students in the Near East. Journal of Educational Psychology, 48, 193–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duit, R. (1991). On the role of analogies and metaphors in learning science. Science Education, 75(6), 649–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedler, Y., Zohar, A., & Tamir, P. (1993). The effect of age and of learning on the ability to distinguish between anthropomorphic and teleological explanations. International Journal of Science Education, 15(4), 439–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallant, R. A. (1981). Pitfalls of personification. Science and Children, 19(2), 16–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glynn, S. M., Britton, B. K., Semrud-Clikeman, M., & Muth, K. D. (1989). Analogical reasoning and problem solving in science textbooks. In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), A handbook of creativity: Assessment, research and theory. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampel, C. (1965). Aspects of scientific explanations. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howard, R. (1989). Teaching science with metaphors. School Science Review, 70(252), 100–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, A. (1973). Anthropomorphism, teleology, animism, and personification-why they should be avoided. Science and Children, 10, 10–11

    Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (1987). Young children's spontaneous personification as analogy. Child Development, 58, 1013–1020.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kallery, M. (2001). Early-years educators' attitudes to science and pseudo-science: The case of astronomy and astrology. European Journal of Teacher Education, 24(3), 329–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kallery, M., & Psillos, D. (2001). Preschool teachers' content knowledge in science: Their understanding of elementary science concepts and of issues raised by children's questions. International Journal of Early Years Education, 9(3), 165–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kallery, M., & Psillos, D. (2002). What happens in the early years science classroom? The reality of teachers curriculum implementation activities. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 10(2), 49–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemke, J. L. (1990). Thinking science: Language learning and values. Norwood, MA: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowrie, D. E. (1954). Additional data on animistic thinking, Scientific Monthly, 79, 69–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Looft, W. R., & Bartz, H. (1969). Animism revived. Psychological Bulletin, 71(1), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papalia-Finlay, D. E. (1978). The life concept in female college students: An exploratory analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 7(2), 133–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1951). The child's conception of the world. Savage, MD: Littlefield Adams.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharefkin, B., & Ruchlis, H. (1974). Anthropomorphismin the lower grades. Science and Children, 11(6), 37–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, L. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching. Educational Researcher, 15(2), 4–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, A. J., & Goss, A. E. (1957). Animistic responses as a function of sentence contexts and instructions. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 91, 181–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taber, K. S. (1995). An analogy for discussing progression in learning chemistry. School Science Review, 76(276), 91–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taber, K. S., & Watts, M. (1996). The secret life of the chemical bond: Students' anthropomorphic and animistic references to bonding. International Journal of Science Education, 18(5), 557–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, P., & Zohar, A. (1991). Anthropomorphism and teleology in reasoning about biological phenomena. Science Education, 75(1), 57–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treagust, D. F., & Harrison, A. G. (2000). In search of explanatory frameworks: An analysis of Richard Feynman's lecture 'Atoms in motion.' International Journal of Science Education, 22(11), 1157–1170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, M., & Bentley, D. (1994). Humanizing and feminising school science: reviving anthropomorphic and animistic thinking in constructivist science education. International Journal of Science Education, 16(1), 83–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zohar, A, & Ginossar, S. (1998). Lifting the taboo regarding teleology and anthropomorphism in biology education-heretical suggestions. Science Education, 82(6), 679–697.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kallery, M., Psillos, D. Anthropomorphism and Animism in Early Years Science: Why Teachers Use Them, how They Conceptualise Them and What Are Their Views on Their Use. Research in Science Education 34, 291–311 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:RISE.0000044613.64634.03

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:RISE.0000044613.64634.03

Navigation