Skip to main content
Log in

Emotion Specificity and Consumer Behavior: Anger, Sadness, and Preference for Activity

  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examine the influence of two specific negative emotions (i.e., sadness and anger) on consumers' preference for an advertised product promoting either activity (e.g., exercise) or passivity (e.g., relaxation). On the basis of empirical distinctions between the level of activation accompanying sadness and anger, and drawing upon a mood-as-information perspective, we hypothesized that individuals will have a preference for activity to passivity when in an angry compared to a sad emotional state. Thus, when angry, they preferred a product advertised as active, whereas when sad they preferred a product described in more passive terms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Aristotle (1991). Aristotle, On Rhetoric: A Theory of Civic Discourse (G. A. Kennedy, Trans.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, L., & Troccolil, B. T. (1990). Feelings, direction of attention, and expressed evaluations of others. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 305-325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., Bohner, G., Schwarz, N., & Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 331-345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 445-462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought: Limits of awareness, intention, and control (pp. 212-252). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., Gasper, K., & Garvin, E. (2001). Affect as information. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 121-144). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. B., & Areni, C. S. (1991). Affect and consumer behavior. In T. S. Robertson & H. H. Kassarjian (Eds.), Handbook of consumer behavior (pp. 188-240). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Rucker, D. D., Wegener, D. T., & Braverman, J. (2004). Discrete emotions and persuasion: The role of emotion-induced expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 43-56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Wegener, D. T., & Rucker, D. D. (2000). Beyond valence in the perception of likelihood: The role of emotion-specificity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 397-416.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erber, M. W., & Erber, R. (2001). The role of motivated social cognition in the regulation of affective states. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 275-290). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erber, R., & Tesser, A. (1992). Task effort and the regulation of mood: The absorption hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 339-359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, Barrett, L., & Russell, J. A. (1998). Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 967-984.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (Ed). (1991). Emotion and social judgments. Oxford, England: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P., & Moylan, S. (1987). After the movies: Transient moods and social judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 467-477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, M. P. (1985). Mood states and consumer behavior: A Critical Review. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 281-300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Seligman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 797-803.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (1990). Regional brain electrical asymmetries discriminate between previously depressed and healthy control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 22-31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (1991). Left frontal hypoactivation in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 535-545.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition, (Vol. 3, pp. 179-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, W. B. G. (2000). MediaLab 2000 [Computer software]. New York: Empirisoft.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, E. J., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 20-31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P. C., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 740-752.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146-159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, J. D., Gaschke, Y. N., Braverman, D. L., & Evans, T. W. (1992). Mood-congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 119-132.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, W. J. (1985). Attitudes and attitude change. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd ed. Vol. 2, pp. 233-346). New York, NY: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nabi, R. (2002). Anger, fear, uncertainty, and attitudes: A test of the cognitive-functional model. Communication Monographs, 69, 204-216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nabi, R. (2003). Exploring the framing effects of emotion: Do discrete emotions differentially influence information accessibility, information seeking, and policy preference? Communication Research, 30, 224-247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ottati, V. C., & Isbell, L. M. (1996). Effects of mood during exposure to target information on subsequently reported judgments: An on-line model of misattribution and correction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 39-53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E. (1997). The evolution of theory and research in social psychology: From single to multiple effect and process models. In C. McGarty & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The message of social psychology: Perspectives on mind in society (pp. 268-290). Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1979). Issue involvement can increase or decrease persuasion by enhancing message-relevant cognitive responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1915-1926.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123-205). New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kasmer, J. (1988). The role of affect in the Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion. In L. Donohew, H. Sypher, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Communication, social cognition, and affect (pp. 117-146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., DeSteno, D., & Rucker, D. D. (2001). The role of affect in attitude change. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 212-233). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Fabrigar, L. R., & Wegener, D. T., (2003). Emotional factors in attitudes and persuasion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 752-772). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Gleicher, F., & Baker, S. M. (1991). Multiple roles for affect in persuasion. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgments (pp. 181-200). Oxford, England: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Priester, J. R., & Briñol, P. (2002). Mass media attitude change: Implications of the Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd ed. pp. 155-198). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., Schumann, D. W., Richman, S. A., & Strathman, A. J. (1993). Positive mood and persuasion: Different roles for affect under high-and low-elaboration conditions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5-20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Attitude change: Multiple roles for persuasion variables. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 323-390). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pham, M. (1998). Representativeness, relevance, and the use of feelings in decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 144-159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79, 56-77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Razran, G. H. S. (1940). Conditioned response changes in rating and appraising sociopolitical slogans. Psychological Bulletin, 37, 481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. W. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.), Social psychophysiology: A sourcebook (pp. 153-176). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2004). Ready for action? Anger, sadness, and preference for activity. Unpublished manuscript, Ohio State University.

  • Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, (Vol. 2, pp. 527-561). New York, NY: The Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L., (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 512-523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feeling and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 433-465). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., Schwarz, N., & Gschneidinger (1985). Happiness and reminiscing: The role of time perspective, affect, and mode of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1450-1469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E. (1991). The asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67-85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 973-988.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1994). Mood-management across affective states: The hedonic contingency hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1034-1048.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1997). The flexible correction model: The role of naïve theories of bias in bias correction. In. M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 141-208). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., & Klein, D. J. (1994). Effects of mood on high elaboration attitude change: The mediating role of likelihood judgments. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 25-44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., & Smith, S. M. (1995). Positive mood can increase or decrease message scrutiny: The hedonic contingency view of mood and message processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 5-15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, W., & Fine, B. J. (1956). The effect of induced aggressiveness on opinion change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 109-114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zanna, M. P., Kiesler, C. A., & Pilkonis, P. A. (1970). Positive and negative attitudinal affect established by classical conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 321-328.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rucker, D.D., Petty, R.E. Emotion Specificity and Consumer Behavior: Anger, Sadness, and Preference for Activity. Motivation and Emotion 28, 3–21 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:MOEM.0000027275.95071.82

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:MOEM.0000027275.95071.82

Navigation