Abstract
This article reports the utility of an information processing approach to examine whether there is a relationship between sexual content induced delay and levels of sexual desire as determined by self-report questionnaires. We tested this idea using a partial replication of the J. H. Geer and H. S. Bellard (1996) protocol demonstrating sexual content induced delay (SCID) in responding to sexual versus neutral words. In addition, the experiment examined whether SCID was different in people with varying levels of sexual desire. It was hypothesized that persons with low levels of sexual desire might respond more slowly to sexual word cues than others. Words with equal frequency of usage and similar word length were chosen from among those used in the Geer and Bellard study. The experiment was conducted with 171 volunteers who completed sexual desire questionnaires, lexical decision making tasks, and word ratings. The SCID effect was demonstrated by both men and women in the study with no significant variation between the sexes. In accordance with prediction, it was found that persons with lower levels of sexual desire responded more slowly to sexual stimuli than other participants, and rated sexual words as less familiar, less acceptable, and less positive emotionally to them. These findings have implications for understanding how emotional content contributes to SCID. They also suggest that further exploration of these ideas, perhaps using other stimulus modalities, may be helpful in advancing understanding of responses to sexual cues, and the potential implications that may have in better understanding sexual desire.
Similar content being viewed by others
REFERENCES
Apt, C., & Hurlbert, D. F. (1992). Motherhood and female sexuality beyond one year postpartum: A study of military wives. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 18, 104-114.
Apt, C., Hurlbert, D. F., Sarmiento, G. R., & Hurlbert, M. K. (1996). The role of fellatio in marital sexuality: An examination of sexual compatibility and sexual desire. Sexual and Marital Therapy, 11, 383-392.
Balota, D. A. (1994). Visualword recognition: The journey from features to meaning. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 303-357). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Barlow, D. H. (1986). Causes of sexual dysfunction: The role of anxiety and cognitive interference. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 140-148.
Beck, J. G., & Bozman, A. W. (1995). Gender differences in sexual desire: The effects of anger and anxiety. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24, 595-612.
Conaglen, H. M., & Evans, I. M. (2004). Pictorial cues and sexual desire: An experimental approach to understanding desire. Manuscript under review.
Everaerd, W. (1995). Information processing approach and the sexual response in human studies. In J. Bancroft (Ed.), The pharmacology of sexual function and dysfunction (pp. 175-184). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Farkas, G. M., Sine, L. F., & Evans, I. M. (1979). The effects of distraction, performance demand, stimulus explicitness, and personality on objective and subjective measures of male sexual arousal. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 17, 25-32.
Francis, W. N., & Kucera, H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English usage: Lexicon and grammar. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Geer, J. H. (1996). Gender differences in the organization of sexual information. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 91-107.
Geer, J. H., & Bellard, H. S. (1996). Sexual content induced delays in unprimed lexical decisions: Gender and context effects. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 379-395.
Geer, J. H., & Fuhr, R. (1976). Cognitive factors in sexual arousal: The role of distraction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 238-243.
Geer, J. H., Judice, S. L., & Jackson, S. R. (1994). Reading times of erotic material: The pause to reflect. Journal of General Psychology, 121, 345-352.
Geer, J. H., Lapour, K. J., & Jackson, S. R. (1993). The information processing approach to human sexuality. In N. Birbaumer & A. Ohman (Eds.), The structure of emotion: Psychophysiological, cognitive and clinical aspects (pp. 139-155). Toronto: Hogrefe-Huber.
Geer, J. H., & Manguno-Mire, G. M. (1996). Gender differences in cognitive processes in sexuality. Annual Review of Sex Research, 7, 90-124.
Geer, J. H., & McGlone, M. S. (1990). Sex differences in memory for erotica. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 71-78.
Geer, J. H., & Melton, J. S. (1997). Sexual content-induced delay with double-entendre words. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26, 295-316.
Gernsbacher, M. A. (Ed.). (1994). Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Janssen, E., Everaerd,W., Spiering, M., & Janssen, J. (2000). Automatic processes and the appraisal of sexual stimuli:Toward an information processing model of sexual arousal. Journal of Sex Research, 37, 8-23.
Leiblum, S. R., & Rosen, R. C. (1988). Introduction: Changing perspectives on sexual desire. In S. R. Leiblum & R. C. Rosen (Eds.), Sexual desire disorders (pp. 1-17). New York: Guilford.
Spector, I. P., Carey, M. P., & Steinberg, L. (1996). The Sexual Desire Inventory: Development, factor structure, and evidence of reliability. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 22, 175-190.
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Conaglen, H.M. Sexual Content Induced Delay: A Reexamination Investigating Relation to Sexual Desire. Arch Sex Behav 33, 359–367 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:ASEB.0000028889.63425.fb
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:ASEB.0000028889.63425.fb