Sex Roles

, Volume 51, Issue 3–4, pp 117–128 | Cite as

Understanding Subtle Sexism: Detection and Use of Sexist Language

  • Janet K. Swim
  • Robyn Mallett
  • Charles Stangor

Abstract

In the present research we examined the association between Modern Sexist beliefs and identifying and engaging in subtle sexist behavior. In Study 1, we found that those who endorsed Modern Sexist beliefs were less likely to detect the occurrence of normative sexist behavior (i.e., the use of sexist language), and this oversight was a function of their failure to define such behavior as sexist. In Study 2, we found that those who endorsed Modern Sexist beliefs were more likely to use sexist language and less likely to use nonsexist language. Use of nonsexist language was a function of personal definitions of sexist language. Results are discussed in terms of motivations to self-correct discriminatory behavior and conceptualizations of current forms of sexism.

sexist language modern sexism subtle sexism 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

REFERENCES

  1. Banaji, M. R., & Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping.Psychological Science, 7 ,136–141.Google Scholar
  2. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 ,1173–1182.Google Scholar
  3. Benokraitis, N. V., & Feagin, J. R. (1999). Modern sexism (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  4. Blair, I. V. (2001). Implicit stereotypes and prejudice. In G. B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition (pp. 359–374). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Bodenhausen, G. V., & Macrae, C. N. (1996). The self-regulation of intergroup perception: Mechanisms and consequences of stereotype suppression. In C. N. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping (pp. 227–253). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  6. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Crawford, M. (2001). Gender and language. In R. K. Unger (Ed.), Handbook of the psychology of women and gender (pp. 228–244). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  8. Crocker, J., Voelkl, K. Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60 ,218–228.Google Scholar
  9. Cronin, C., & Jreisat, S. (1995). Effects of modeling on the use of nonsexist language among high school freshpersons and seniors. Sex Roles, 33 ,819–830.Google Scholar
  10. Devine, P. G., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Vance, S. L. (2002). The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: The role of motivations to respond without prejudice. J ournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82,835–848.Google Scholar
  11. Gay, W. C. (1997). The reality of linguistic violence against women. In L. L. O'Toole & J. R. Schiffman(Eds.), Gender violence: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 467–473). New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 ,491–512.Google Scholar
  13. Hu, L., & Bentler, P.M. (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 1 ,1–55.Google Scholar
  14. Hyde, J. S. (1984). Children's understanding of sexist language. Developmental Psychology, 20 ,697–706.Google Scholar
  15. Kennedy, D. (1993). Nonsexist language: A progress report: Against the grain: Narratives of resistance [Special issue]. Canadian Journal of Education, 18 ,223–238.Google Scholar
  16. Keogh, E., Ellery, D., Hunt, C., & Hannent, I. (2001). Selective attentional bias for pain-related stimuli amongst pain fearful individuals. Pain, 91 ,91–100.Google Scholar
  17. Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social-Psychology, 78 ,871–888.Google Scholar
  18. Lips, H. M. (1997). Sex and gender: An introductio (3rd ed.). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.Google Scholar
  19. Maass, A., & Arcuri, L. (1996). Language and stereotyping. In C. N. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping (pp. 193–227). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  20. MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure madeling. Psychological methods, 1 ,130–149.Google Scholar
  21. MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test sers2004.cls (04/06/2004 v1.1 LaTeX2e SERS document class) pp1296-sers-491456 August 5, 2004 18:8 mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7 ,83–104.Google Scholar
  22. Monteith, M. J. (1993). Self-regulation of prejudiced responses: Implications for progress in prejudice-reduction efforts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65 ,469–485.Google Scholar
  23. Monteith, M. J., & Voils, C. I. (2001). Exerting control over prejudiced responses. In G. B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition (pp. 375–388).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  24. McConnell, A. R., & Fazio, R. H. (1996). Women as men and people: Effects of gender-marked language. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22 ,1004–1013.Google Scholar
  25. McMinn, M. R., Lindsay, S. F., Hannum, L. E., & Troyer, P. K. (1990). Does sexist language reflect personal characteristics? Sex Roles, 23 ,389–396.Google Scholar
  26. McMinn, M. R., Troyer, P. K., Hannum, L. E., & Foster, J. (1991). Teaching nonsexist language to college students. Journal of Experimental Education, 59 ,153–161.Google Scholar
  27. McMinn, M. R., Williams, P. E., & McMinn, L. C. (1994). Assessing recognition of sexist language: Development and use of the Gender-Specific Language Scale. Sex Roles, 31 ,741–755.Google Scholar
  28. Parks, J. B., & Roberton, M. A. (1998). Contemporary arguments against nonsexist language: Blaubergs (1980) revisited. Sex Roles, 39 ,445–461.Google Scholar
  29. Rudman, L. A., & Kilianski, S. E. (2000). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward female authority. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26 ,1315–1328.Google Scholar
  30. Ruscher, J. B. (2001). Prejudicedcommunication: A social psychological perspective. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  31. Sears, D. O. (1988). Symbolic racism. In P. A. Katz & D. A. Taylor(Eds.), Eliminating racism: Profiles in controversy (pp. 53–84). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  32. Segerstrom, S. C. (2001). Optimism and attentional bias for negative and positive stimuli. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27 ,1334–1343.Google Scholar
  33. Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1973). A short version of the Attitudes Toward Women Scale (AWS). Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 2 ,219–220.Google Scholar
  34. Stangor, C., Swim, J. K., Sechrist, G. G., DeCoster, J. Van Allen, K. L., & Ottenbreit, A. (2003). Ask, Answer and Announce: Three Stages in Perceiving and responding to Discrimination. European Review of Social Psychology, 14 ,277–311.Google Scholar
  35. Stewart, M. W., Verstraate, C. D., & Fanslow, J. L. (1990). Sexist language and university academic staff: Attitudes, awareness, and recognition of sexist language. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies, 25 ,115–125.Google Scholar
  36. Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and racism: Old-fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68 ,199–214.Google Scholar
  37. Swim, J. K., & Campbell, B. (2001). Sexism: Attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors. In R. Brown & S. Gaertner (Eds.), The Handbook of social psychology: Intergroup relations (Vol. 4, pp. 218–237). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  38. Swim, J. K., & Cohen, L. L. (1997). Overt, covert, and subtle sexism: A comparison between the Attitudes Toward Women and Modern Sexism Scales. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21 ,103–118.Google Scholar
  39. Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J. (2001). Everyday sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature, and psychological impact from three daily diary studies. Journal of Social Issues, 57 ,31–53.Google Scholar
  40. Swim, J. K., & Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., Fitzgerald, D. F., & Bylsma, W. B. (2003). African American college students' experiences with everyday anti-Black racism: Characteristics of and responses to these incidents. Journal of Black Psychology, 29 ,38–67.Google Scholar
  41. Swim, J. K., Mallet, R., Russo-Devosa, Y.,& Stangor, C. (in press). Subtle sexism: An assessment of gender related beliefs and sexist behaviors. Psychology of Women Quarterly.Google Scholar
  42. Swim, J. K., Scott, E., Sechrist, G. B., Campbell, B., & Stangor, C. (2003). The role of intent and harm in judgments of prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84 ,944–959.Google Scholar
  43. Tougas, F., Brown, R., Beaton, A. M., & Joly, S. (1995). Neosexism: Plus ça change, plus c'est pareil. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 ,842–849.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Janet K. Swim
    • 1
  • Robyn Mallett
    • 1
  • Charles Stangor
    • 2
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentThe Pennsylvania State University
  2. 2.Psychology DepartmentThe University of Maryland

Personalised recommendations