Skip to main content
Log in

Future-Event Schemas: Automaticity and Rumination in Major Depression

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined the proposition that individuals with major depression make predictions about future events relatively automatically and pessimistically, reflecting use of a future-event schema, while they also ruminate about the future. Depressed participants and nondepressed controls indicated whether or not various positive and negative future events would happen to them or to an average other—either under a concurrent attentional load or no such load—while their response latencies were assessed. As hypothesized, depressives showed relatively greater automaticity in their predictions than did nondepressives, and a lack of optimism as well. More specifically, depressives showed a smaller increase in response latency due to the introduction of the attentional load than did nondepressives, suggesting relatively greater processing efficiency, and they also predicted reliably fewer positive events. Indeed, depressives also reported ruminating more about the future based on a recent distressing life event. Overall, the results extend research on future-event schemas and automaticity (Andersen, Spielman, & Bargh, 1992) from moderate to major depression and establish a link with future-event rumination.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Ahrens, A. H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions for self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 366–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

  • Andersen, S. M. (1990). The inevitability of future suffering: The role of depressive predictive certainty in depression. Social Cognition, 8, 203–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. M., & Lyon, J. E. (1987). Anticipating undesired outcomes: The role of outcome certainty in the onset of depressive affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 428–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. M., & Schwartz, A. H. (1992). Intolerance of ambiguity and depression: A cognitive vulnerability factor linked to hopelessness. Social Cognition, 10, 271–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, S. M., Spielman, L. A., & Bargh, J. A. (1992). Future-event schemas and certainty about the future: Automaticity in depressives' future-event predictions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 711–723.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr., & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 1–40), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Tota, M. E. (1988). Content-dependent automatic processing in depression: Accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 925–939.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Hoeber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1974). Cognitions, affect, and psychopathology. In H. London & R. E. Nisbett (Eds.), Thought and feeling: Cognitive alteration of feeling states (pp. 127–140). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. Madison, CT: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. E., & Erbaugh, J. K. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, J., & Weissman, M. (1981). Epidemiology of affective disorders: A reexamination and future directions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 1039–1046.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. G., & Harris, T. O. (1978). Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C. (1994). Self-reported distress: Analog or ersatz depression? Psychological Bulletin, 116, 29–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression:Acritical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deardoff, W. W., & Funabiki, D. (1985).Adiagnostic caution in screening for depressed college students. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 227–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. L. (1978). A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837–844.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franche, R. L., & Dobson, K. (1992). Self-criticism and interpersonal dependency as vulnerability factors to depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 419–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garber, J., Miller, S. M., & Abramson, L. Y. (1980). On the distinction between anxiety and depression: Perceived control, certainty, and probability of goal attainment. In J. Garber and M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Human helplessness: Theory and applications (pp. 131–169). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, F. X., Smith, T. W., Ingram, R. E., Pearce, K., Brehm, S. S., & Schroeder, D. (1985). Selfawareness and self-confrontation: Effects of self-focused attention on members of a clinical population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 662–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goethe, J. W., & Fischer, E. H. (1995). Functional impairment in depressed inpatients.Journal of Affective Disorders, 33, 23–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldston, D. B., O'Hara, M. W., & Schartz, H. A. (1990). Reliability, validity, and preliminary normative data for the Inventory to Diagnose Depression in a college population. Psychological Assessment, 2, 212–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H. (1984). Depression and general psychopathology in university students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 19–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haaga, D. A. F., Dyck, M. J., & Ernst, D. (1991). Empirical status of cognitive theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 215–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammen, C., Marks, T., de Mayo, R., & Mayol, A. (1985). Self-schemas and risk for depression: A prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1147–1159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartlage, S., Alloy, L. B., Vazquez, C., & Dykman, B. (1993). Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 247–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T., & Hardin, T. S. (1990). Remembering with and without awareness in a depressed mood: Evidence of deficits in initiative. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 45–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social perception. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 369–425). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, E. T., Van Hook, E., & Dorfman, D. (1988). Do self-attributes form a cognitive structure? Social Cognition, 6, 177–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A. B., & Knowles, T. H. (1991). Depression and the “emotional” Stroop effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 481–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollon, S. D., & Garber, J. (1980). A cognitive- expectancy theory of therapy for helplessness and depression. In J. Garber & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Helplessness: Theory and applications (pp. 173–195). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, R. E., Lumry, A. E., Cruet, D., & Sieber, W. (1987). Attentional processes in depressive disorders. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 351–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantowitz, B. H. (1974). Double stimulation. In B. H. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition (pp. 83–132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., & Ingram, R.E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding the use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall, P. C., & Ingram, R. E. (1989). Cognitive- behavioral perspectives: Theory and research on depression and anxiety. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds.), Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 27–53). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Cowan, G. S. (1988). Internal focus of attention and depression: A study of daily experience. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 237–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. S., & Averill, J. R. (1972). Emotion and cognition with special reference to anxiety. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 2, pp. 242–283). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1979). On the use of a concurrent memory load to measure attention and automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 189–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Self-perpetuating properties of dysphoric rumination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 339–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, A. K., & Cropley, M. L. (1995). Depressive future-thinking: The role of valence and specificity. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 35–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • McFarlane, A. H., Bellissimo, A., & Norman, G. R. (1995). The role of family and peers in social selfefficacy: Links to depression in adolescence. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 402–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyers, J., Weissman, M., Tischler, G., Holzer, C., Leaf, P., Orvaschel, H., Anthony, J., Boyd, J., Burke, J., Kramer, M., & Stolzman, R. (1984). Six-month prevalence of psychiatric disorders in three communities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 959–967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1990). Effects of responses to depression on the remediation of depressive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 519–527.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musson, R. F., & Alloy, L. B. (1988). Depression and self-directed attention. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 193–220). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and post-traumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1993). Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 561–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 92–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., & Bolton, W. (1985). A social- cognitive theory of depression in reaction to life events. Psychological Review, 92, 372–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pietromonaco, P. R., & Markus, H. (1985). The nature of negative thoughts in depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 799–807.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prud'homme, L., & Barron, P. (1992). The pattern of irrational beliefs associated with major depressive disorder. Social Behavior and Personality, 20, 199–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., Hamilton, J. H., Herring, F., & Greenberg, J. (1989). Depression, self-focused attention, and the negative memory bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 351–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyszczynski, T., Holt, K., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Depression, self-focused attention, and expectancies for positive and negative future life events for self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 994–1001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 510–532.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reich, D. A., & Weary, G. (1998). The role of future-events schemas on social inference processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1133–1145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riskind, J. H., Rholes, W. S., Brannon, A. M., & Burdick, C. A. (1987). Attributions and expectations: A confluence of vulnerabilities in mild depression in a college student population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 349–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. E., Croughan, J., & Ratcliff, K. S. (1981). National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: Its history, characteristics, and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 381–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlager, D., Froom, J., & Jaffe, A. (1995). Winter depression and functional impairment among ambulatory primary care patients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 36, 18–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A., & Bjork, R. A. (1992). New conceptualizations of practice: Common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3, 207–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, L., & O'Hara, M. W. (1993). Self-discrepancies in clinically anxious and depressed university students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 282–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V. (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive models of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 147–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z., Gemar, M., Truchon, C., Guirguis, M., & Horowitz, L. M. (1995). A priming methodology for studying self-representation in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 205–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V., Hood, J. E., Shaw, B. F., & Higgins, E. T. (1988). A structural analysis of the self-schema construct in major depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 12, 471–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R. (1989). Procedural efficiency: General and specific components and effects on social judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 500–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R. (1994). Procedural knowledge and processing strategies in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 99–151). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. W., Ingram, R. E., & Roth, L. D. (1985). Self-focused attention and depression: Self-evaluation, affect, and life stress. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 381–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tait, R., & Silver, R. C. (1989). Coming to terms with major negative life events. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 351–382). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., Taylor, M. J., Cooper, Z., Hayhurst, H., & Paykel, E. S. (1995). Depressive thinking: Shifts in construct accessibility or in schematic mental models? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 500–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennen, H., Hall, J. A., & Affleck, G. (1995). Depression research methodologies in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: A review and critique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 870–884.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veiel, H. O. F. (1990). Detrimental effects of kin support networks on the course of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 419–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vredenburg, K., Flett, G. L., & Krames, L. (1993). Analogue versus clinical depression: A critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 327–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weary, G., & Edwards, J. A. (1994). Social cognition and clinical psychology: Anxiety, depression, and the processing of social information. In R. S. Wyer, Jr., & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 289–338). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williamson, P. S., & Yates, W. R. (1989). The initial presentation of depression in family practice and psychiatric outpatients. General Hospital Psychiatry, 11, 188–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. (1987). The inventory to diagnose depression (IDD): A self-report scale to diagnose major depressive disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 55–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. (1988). The validity of a self-report questionnaire for diagnosing major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45, 738–740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, M., & Coryell, W. (1994). Screening for major depressive disorder in the community: A comparison of measures. Psychological Assessment, 6, 71–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, M., Coryell, W., Corenthal, C., & Wilson, S. (1986). A self-report scale to diagnose major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 1076–1081.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuppan, B. A., Hammen, C., & Jaenicke, C. (1987). The effects of current mood and prior depressive history on self-schematic processing in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 149–158.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Andersen, S.M., Limpert, C. Future-Event Schemas: Automaticity and Rumination in Major Depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research 25, 311–333 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026447600924

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026447600924

Navigation