Skip to main content
Log in

Threat-Evoked Anxiety Disrupts Spatial Working Memory Performance: An Attentional Account

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is proposed that threat-evoked anxiety and spatial Working Memory (WM) rely on a common visuospatial attention mechanism. A prediction of this hypothesis is that spatial but not verbal WM should be disrupted in conditions of threat anxiety. Participants performed verbal and spatial n-back WM tasks in the presence or absence of threat of shock (shocks were not delivered). The presence of anxiety was assessed via heart rate recordings and self-report. Both measures clearly distinguished between WM blocks associated with threat of shock (Threat) and blocks, in which threat was absent (Safety). Performance on the spatial WM task was impaired in Threat relative to Safety. Furthermore, the more anxiety participants reported and the higher their heart rate in Threat compared to Safety, the more impaired was their spatial WM performance. This effect was not observed for verbal WM. The results indicate selective disruption of spatial WM performance by threat-evoked anxiety, interpreted in terms of more overlap in visuospatial attention between anxiety and spatial WM vs. anxiety and verbal WM.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review, 106, 529-550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Awh, E., Jonides, J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Rehearsal in spatial working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 780-790.

    Google Scholar 

  • Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Buxton, R. B., Frank, L. R., Love, T., et al. (1999). Rehearsal in spatial working memory: Evidence from neuroimaging. Psychological Science, 10, 433-437.

    Google Scholar 

  • Awh, E., Lourdes, A. V., & Hillyard, S. A. (2000). The role of spatial selective attention in working memory for locations: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 840-847.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ax, A. F. (1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433-442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation(pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartolic, E. I., Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Glauser, T., & Titanic-Schefft, M. (1999). Effects of experimentally-induced emotional stateson frontal lobe cognitive task performance. Neuropsychologia, 37, 677-683.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, A., & Lader, M. (1974). The use of analog scales in rating subjective feelings. British Journal of Medical Psychiatry, 47, 211-218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkovec, T. D., & Inz, J. (1990). The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: A predominance of thought activity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 153-158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkovec, T. D., Lyonfields, J. D., Wiser, S. L., & Diehl, I. (1993). The role of worrisome thinking in the suppression of cardiovascular response to phobic imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 321-324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchwald, A. M., Stark, S., & Coyne, C. (1981). Demand characteristics and the Velten mood induction procedure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 478-479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1-47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chua, P., Krams, M., Toni, I., Passingham, R., & Dolan, R. (1999). A functional anatomy of anticipatory anxiety. Neuroimage, 9, 563-571.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J. (1992). Anterior cerebral assymetry and the nature of emotion. Brain and Cognition, 20, 125-151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derakshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (1998). Working Memory capacity in high trait-anxious and repressor groups. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 697-713.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1987). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affect, cognition and social behaviour. Toronto: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1979). Anxiety, learning, and memory: A reconceptualization. Journal of Research in Personality, 13, 363-385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1982). Attention and arousal: Cognition and performance. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409-434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gevins, A., Smith, M. E., Le, J., Leong, H., Bennett, J., Martin, N., et al. (1996). High resolution evoked potential imaging of the cortical dynamics of human WM. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 98, 327-348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. R. (2001). Emotional modulation of cognitive control: Approach-withdrawal states double-dissociate spatial from verbal 2-back task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 436-452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., & Raichle, M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 99, 4115-4120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grillon, C., Ameli, R., Woods, S.W., Merikangas, K., & Davis, M. (1991). Fear-potentiated startle in humans: Effects of anticipatory anxiety in the acoustic startle blink reflex. Psychophysiology, 28, 588-595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, W., Etienne, M. A., & Miller, G. A. (1995). Patterns of perceptual asymmetry in depression and anxiety: Implications for neuropsychological models of emotion and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 327-333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, W., & Nitschke, J. B. (1998). The puzzle of regional brain activity in depression and anxiety: The importance of subtypes and comorbidity. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 421-447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ikeda, M., Iwanaga, M., & Seiwa, H. (1996). Test anxiety and working memory system. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 1223-1231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122-1131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavric, A., Shackman, A. J., Sarinopoulos, I., Pederson, A., and Davidson, R. J. (2000). Effects of threat-of shock upon verbal and spatial working memory. Psychophysiology, 37, S62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markham, R., & Darke, S. (1991). The effects of anxiety on verbal and spatial task performance. Australian Journal of Psychology, 43, 107-111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 539-560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., & Fulcher, E. P. (1997). Cognitive biases in anxiety and attention to threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 341-346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive–motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 809-848.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford, M., Morris, F., Grainger, B., & Williams, J. M. G. (1996). Mood, reasoning and central executive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 476-492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palomba, D., Sarlo, M., Angrilli, A., Mini, A., & Stegagno, L. (2000). Cardiac responses associated with affective processing of unpleasant film stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 36, 45-57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philippot, P. (1993). Inducing and assessing differentiated emotion-feeling states in the laboratory. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 171-193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rauch, S. L., Savage, C. R., Alpert, N. M., Fischman, A. J., & Jenike, M. A. (1997). The functional neuroanatomy anxiety: A study of three disorders using Positron Emission Tomography and symptom provocation. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 446-452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiman, E. M., Fusselman, M. J., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1989). Neuroanatomical correlates of anxiety. Science, 243, 1071-1074.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetza, C., et al. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 174-187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1993). Neuroscience projections to current debates in emotion psychology. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 1-41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shackman, A. J., Slagter, H. A., Lavric, A., Irwin, W., Sarinopoulos, I. Oakes T. R., et al. (in press). Hemispheric asymmetry of verbal and spatial working memory in prefrontal cortex. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts.

  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283, 1657-1661.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lavric, A., Rippon, G. & Gray, J.R. Threat-Evoked Anxiety Disrupts Spatial Working Memory Performance: An Attentional Account. Cognitive Therapy and Research 27, 489–504 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026300619569

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026300619569

Navigation